
Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Evidence from an fMRI study
Tingting Wang
Lei Mo
Ce Mo
Li Hai Tan
Jonathan S Cant
Luojin Zhong
Gerald Cupchik
Correspondence should be addressed to Lei Mo, Department of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, China. E-mail:molei@scnu.edu.cn
Corresponding author.
Received 2014 Feb 22; Revised 2014 Aug 20; Accepted 2014 Sep 17; Issue date 2015 Jun.
Abstract
Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Two functional magnetic resonance imaging experiments were performed to answer this question. Experiment 1 investigated the network of moral aesthetic judgments and facial aesthetic judgments. Participants performed aesthetic judgments and gender judgments on both faces and scenes containing moral acts. The conjunction analysis of the contrasts ‘facial aesthetic judgment > facial gender judgment’ and ‘scene moral aesthetic judgment > scene gender judgment’ identified the common involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), inferior temporal gyrus and medial superior frontal gyrus, suggesting that both types of aesthetic judgments are based on the orchestration of perceptual, emotional and cognitive components. Experiment 2 examined the network of facial beauty and moral beauty during implicit perception. Participants performed a non-aesthetic judgment task on both faces (beautifulvs common) and scenes (containing morally beautifulvs neutral information). We observed that facial beauty (beautiful faces > common faces) involved both the cortical reward region OFC and the subcortical reward region putamen, whereas moral beauty (moral beauty scenes > moral neutral scenes) only involved the OFC. Moreover, compared with facial beauty, moral beauty spanned a larger-scale cortical network, indicating more advanced and complex cerebral representations characterizing moral beauty.
Keywords: facial beauty, fMRI, moral beauty, OFC, putamen
INTRODUCTION
Imagine two scenarios. In the first, you are shocked from viewing a stunning beauty. In the second, you are touched for seeing that a little boy is taking off his coat to cover an injured pigeon. These two scenes seem quite irrelevant to each other. However, they both involve the aesthetic experience of beauty: facial beauty and moral beauty.
According to aesthetic theories, a sense for beauty involves the disinterested and detached mental pleasure induced by various forms, including natural, artistic and moral beauty (Berlyne, 1971;Cupchik, 2002;Haidtet al., 2004). Both natural beauty and artistic beauty are directly based on the auditory pathway and/or visual pathway. It implies that the sensory and symbolic elements of aesthetic objects conform to certain aesthetic rules (e.g. proportion, color and texture) (Berlyne, 1971;Cupchik, 2002). While audiovisual beauty combines sensory and symbolic elements, moral beauty involves complex social meanings and is therefore more complex and abstract (Haidt, 2003a,b,2007;Keltner and Haidt, 2003;Haidtet al., 2004;Diessneret al., 2006;Gu, 2008;Algoe and Haidt, 2009). By definition, moral beauty is the expression of humanity, virtue and talents independent of perceivable physical forms, which is based on the understanding of social rules and involves highly developed social affects and cognitions (Haidt, 2003a,b,2007;Keltner and Haidt, 2003;Haidtet al., 2004;Diessneret al., 2006;Gu, 2008;Algoe and Haidt, 2009). It is noteworthy that although moral beauty may objectively refer to the same human act as moral goodness, subjectively they are different (Haidt, 2003a,b,2007;Keltner and Haidt, 2003). An observer can cognitively judge an act as morally good but remain unmoved. However, when an observer perceives an act as morally beautiful, it implies that his or her emotion has been moved by the act (Haidt, 2003a,b,2007;Keltner and Haidt, 2003). Therefore, exploring the neural mechanisms of moral beauty is of great importance not only in filling the gap in neuroaesthetics but also in establishing a bridge between moral and aesthetic areas (Greeneet al., 2001,2004;Agnatiet al., 2007;Haidt, 2007;Chapmanet al., 2009;Greene and Paxton, 2009;James, 2011;Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011;Zaidel and Nadal, 2011;Avramet al., 2012).
For neuroaesthetics, previous studies have mainly focused on two aspects. The first is to identify the cortical network involved in aesthetic judgments. Aesthetic judgments refer to the evaluation process during appreciation. Irrespective of the beauty level of the stimulus, aesthetic judgments occur when people try to understand and appreciate certain aesthetic objects. Previous studies have identified the neural substrates of aesthetic processing by directly comparing the brain areas underlying aesthetic judgmentsvs non-aesthetic judgments. Several lines of neural evidence have converged on the consistent involvement of the ventral visual systems [V1, V2, V4 and inferior temporal gyrus (ITG)], superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in aesthetic judgments (Jacobsenet al., 2006;Winstonet al., 2007;Avramet al., 2012). The reliance on these regions is in agreement with aesthetic theories, which pose that aesthetic judgments consist of perceptual, cognitive and emotional processing (Berlyne, 1971;Cupchik, 2002). The second is to identify the cortical network involved in beauty. ‘Beauty’ refers to the particular valences of aesthetic judgments (e.g. attractive faces and appealing artwork). Thus, beauty represents a specific level of aesthetic objects. Previous research has identified the neural substrates of beauty by directly comparing beautifulvs neutral objects or by parametrically manipulating the levels of beauty. A variety of studies using painting, music, dance, sculpture and architecture have shown that ‘beauty’ is reliably correlated with activity of cortical and subcortical areas implicated in the processing of reward, especially the OFC and the striatum (including caudate, putamen and nucleus accumbens) (Bloodet al., 1999;Aharonet al., 2001;Blood and Zatorre, 2001;Cela-Condeet al., 2004;Kawabata and Zeki, 2004;Vartanian and Goel, 2004;Menon and Levitin, 2005;Calvo-Merinoet al., 2008;Chatterjeeet al., 2009;Cupchiket al., 2009;Di Dioet al., 2011;Ishizu and Zeki, 2011;Vesselet al., 2012).
However, in contrast to the abundant insights gained in these studies, the focus of which has been confined to audiovisual beauty, little is known about the neural correlates of moral beauty. Does the neural mechanism of moral beauty involve more complex and advanced networks than other beauty forms because of its more abstract and complicated social meaning-based nature? Two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments were conducted to answer this question. Facial beauty was chosen as a comparison, as it is the most typical form of audiovisual beauty. Experiment 1 sought to characterize the network of facial aesthetic judgments and moral aesthetic judgments. We used an aesthetic judgment task and a relatively unrelated gender judgment task as a baseline condition (Jacobsenet al., 2006;Winstonet al., 2007;Avramet al., 2012). Hence, the network of facial aesthetic judgments was identified by the contrast of ‘facial aesthetic judgment > facial gender judgment’, whereas the network of moral aesthetic judgments was identified by the contrast of ‘scene moral aesthetic judgment > scene gender judgment’. That is, Experiment 1 focused on the difference between tasks (aestheticvs non-aesthetic). Experiment 2 examined the network of facial beauty and moral beauty during implicit perception. Participants performed a non-aesthetic judgment task on both scenes containing morally beautiful or neutral information, and beautiful faces or common faces. The neural substrates of facial beauty were identified by the contrast of ‘beautiful faces > common faces’, whereas the neural substrates of moral beauty were identified by the contrast of ‘moral beauty scenes > moral neutral scenes’. That is, Experiment 2 focused on the difference between stimuli (beautifulvs common or neutral). Besides, as no aesthetic task was involved, we could examine whether people could appreciate beauty (moral or facial) automatically and implicitly. Based on previous findings, we hypothesized that the moral aesthetic judgments might rely on similar neural substrates with facial aesthetic judgments, but that moral beauty will involve more complex brain networks related to social cognition.
EXPERIMENT 1
Experiment 1 investigated the network of facial aesthetic judgments and moral aesthetic judgments.
Materials and methods
Participants
Twenty-eight (mean age = 22 years, range, 18–25 years; 14 females) healthy, right-handed college students with no history of psychiatric and neurological disorders were recruited as participants. All participants were assessed to ensure normal or corrected to normal vision. Informed consent was obtained from all participants before the experiment according to the requirements of the Ethics Committee of Department of Psychology, South China Normal University.
Stimuli
During the scan, participants viewed two types of stimuli, facial photos for facial beauty and scene drawings for moral beauty. For facial stimuli, 72 photographs of greyscale photographs of non-famous Asian human faces (with only neutral facial expressions) were selected from the photographic databases of South China Normal University and Beijing Normal University, and rated by an independent group of 48 participants on a 7-point scale, forming two equal-sized sets: ‘beautiful’ faces (5.07 ± 0.27) and ‘common’ faces (3.09 ± 0.18) (seeFigure 1). Each set consisted of 18 male and 18 female faces. Facial photographs were digitized at 600 dpi in 8 bit greyscale, spatially downsized and cropped to fit in an oval ‘window’ sized 350 × 470 pixels. The scene drawings were 72 black-and-white drawings, in which the behaviours of cartoon characters in everyday life scenes were described. In the same pilot study, the 48 participants rated 360 candidate drawings based on the degree of moral beauty conveyed in these pictures (e.g. altruistic behaviour), which yielded 36 ‘moral beauty’ scenes (5.67 ± 0.70) and 36 ‘moral neutral’ scenes (4.22 ± 0.42) (seeFigure 1). In both stimulus sets, the main characters in half the drawings were male. Scene drawings were cropped to fit in a square ‘window’ sized 589 × 500 pixels.
Fig. 1.
Examples of stimuli in Experiment 1. Figure (A) and (B) demonstrate two examples of scene drawings. Figure (A) depicts a morally beautiful scene (SB), in which the main character is performing a morally beautiful (i.e. altruistic) behaviour, and Figure (B) demonstrates a morally neutral scene (SN), in which the main character is performing a neutral behavior. Figure (C) is an example of a beautiful face (FB) and Figure (D) is an example of a common face (FC).
Task
During the scan, participants performed the aesthetic judgment task and the gender judgment task on both stimulus types. In the face aesthetic (FA) judgment task, participants were instructed to judge whether the face was beautiful or common. In the scene moral aesthetic (SA) judgment task, participants were instructed to judge whether the scene drawing was morally beautiful or neutral. In the face gender (FG) task, participants were instructed to judge whether the face was male or female. In the scene gender (SG) task, participants were instructed to judge whether there was a male or a female figure in the picture. The aesthetic judgment required participants to make a left-finger response to beautiful faces or moral beauty scenes and a right-finger response to common faces or neutral scenes. The gender judgment task, which served as a controlling baseline, required participants to make a left-finger response to male faces or male main characters and a right-finger response to female faces or female main characters.
Procedure
The combination of the two task types (aestheticvs gender judgment) and the two stimulus types (facevs scene) yielded four conditions. A blocked design with 12 blocks pertaining to each condition was used. Block orders were fixed and counterbalanced across participants. Each block lasted for 15 s and was followed by 10 s of fixation (seeFigure 2). An instruction for task type appeared for 2 s before block onset. Each block included six trials. Within each block, every image was presented for 2 s subsequently after a 500 ms fixation baseline. Participants were instructed to fixate on each image and make judgments, accordingly, using their thumbs on the button box. Each subject had a single functional scan, which lasted ∼25 min.
Fig. 2.
Experimental design and procedure in Experiment 1. Four types of tasks were performed in separate blocks: FA judgments (beautifulvs common), facial gender (FG) judgments (malevs female), moral aesthetic judgments of scenes (SA; morally beautifulvs neutral) and gender judgments of individuals in scenes (SG; malevs female). The neural substrates underlying the aesthetic judgments for facial beauty were identified by the contrasts of ‘FA-FG’. The neural substrates underlying the aesthetic judgments for moral beauty were identified by the contrasts of ‘SA-SG’. Each block lasted for 15 s and was followed by 10 s of fixation. An instruction for task type appeared for 2 s before block onset.
fMRI data acquisition
Whole-brain fMRI data were acquired on a 3T Siemens Trio scanner with 12-channel phase array head coil. Functional images were obtained in one scanning run with a T2*-weighted gradient echo planner imaging sequence, each comprising 32 contiguous axial slices (3 mm thickness, 1 mm gap). The following parameters were used for scanning: Echo Time (TE) = 35 ms; Repetition Time (TR) = 2000 ms; flip angle = 90°; acquisition matrix size = 64 × 64; field of view = 192 mm. In addition, T1-weighted 3D structural image were acquired using a spoiled gradient-recalled sequence in a steady state (voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mm3; TR = 19 ms, TE = 5 ms, flip angle = 20°).
fMRI data analysis
Data analysis was performed using statistical parametric mapping (SPM8) (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). For each participant, the first six volumes, during which a fixation cross was presented, were discarded to allow for T1 equilibration. Images were spatially realigned to the first volume to correct for head movements. No individual runs had >3 mm maximum displacement or >1° rotation. The aligned functional images were first coregistered to the high-resolution T1-weighted structural image, normalized to the standard T1 template volume (MNI) and then resampled with voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm3. The data were then smoothed with an isotropic full-width half-maximum 8 mm Gaussian kernel.
At the single participant level, a general linear model was applied to the fMRI time series where stimulus onset was modelled as single impulse response function and convolved with SPM8’s canonical haemodynamic response function (HRF). We modelled four regressors of interest: FA, SA, FG and SG. The four block types were time-locked to the onset of the block with block duration of 15 s. The instructions for task type (seeFigure 2) were modelled as a regressor of no interest. Six motion parameters estimated during the realignment procedure were included in the model as covariates of no interest. A high-pass filter with a cut-off period of 128 s was used to remove any low-frequency noise. At the first level, analyses were performed individually for each participant and contrast images were subsequently entered into a second-level analysis treating participants as a random factor.
At the group level, analyses were conducted at a voxel threshold ofP < 0.05 (FWE corrected) and a cluster threshold ofP < 0.05 (FWE corrected). We first performed the contrasts of ‘FA > FG’ and ‘SA > SG,’ respectively, using the flexible factorial analysis in SPM8. Based on our main interest of the common brain regions involved in the above two contrasts, we then computed a conjunction between the ‘FA > FG’ and ‘SA > SG’ using the minimum statistic approach (Nicholset al., 2005).
The parameter estimates in the regions of interest (ROI) were obtained by an iterative leave-one-out-cross-validation (LOOCV) analysis, in which all but one participant’s data were used to define the ROI for the left-out participant (Kriegeskorteet al., 2009). Each ROI was defined using a sphere with 10 mm radius centred on the peak voxel. This procedure was repeated 28 times by omitting a different participant each time, and the parameter estimates were then averaged across participants and plotted. As a result of the LOOCV analysis, the data used to define an ROI and the data extracted from this ROI (bar graphs inFigures 3 and6) are independent.
Fig. 3.
The conjunction results of FA judgments and moral aesthetic judgments. Left ITG, left inferior OFC and bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus were found to exhibit a conjunction effect in both contrasts of ‘aesthetic judgment > gender judgment’ for faces and scenes. Mean parameter estimates in left inferior OFC, left ITG and bilateral superior medial frontal gyrus are shown in four conditions: scene aesthetic judgment (SA), scene gender judgment (SG), face aesthetic judgment (FA), and face gender judgment (FG). A LOOCV analysis was performed (see ‘fMRI data analysis’ for details) to ensure that the data examined within each ROI are independent from the procedure used to define the ROI. *P < 0.05.
Fig. 6.
The conjunction results of perception of physical and moral beauty. Left inferior OFC was found to exhibit a conjunction effect in both contrasts of ‘beautiful faces > common faces’ and ‘moral beauty scenes > neutral scenes’. Mean parameter estimates in left inferior OFC are shown in four conditions: SB, SN, FB, and FC.
Results
Behavioral results
We observed no significant differences in the reaction times (RTs) or accuracy between aesthetic judgment and gender judgment for either faces (RTs:t27 = −1.74, Mean Squared Error (MSE) = 0.03,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = 0.45; accuracy:t27 = −1.81, MSE = 49.15,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = 0.34) or scenes (RTs:t27 = 0.08, MSE = 0.02,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = 0.08; accuracy:t27 = −1.61, MSE = 55.00,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = 0.34). Thus, the observed brain activation differences could not be attributed to performance variation confounds across the different task types.
Imaging results
For FA judgments, a contrast of ‘FA > FG’ revealed strong activation in the bilateral medial SFG, left ITG and left inferior OFC (seeTable 1 andSupplementary Figure S1). For moral aesthetic judgments, a contrast of ‘SA > SG’ revealed activation in the bilateral inferior OFC, left SFG, bilateral ITG, left rectus gyrus, left middle cingulate, bilateral middle occipital lobe and right precuneus (seeTable 1 andSupplementary Figure S1).
Table 1.
Peak voxel size for all regions obtained from the whole-brain random-effects contrasts in Experiment 1 (P < 0.05, FWE corrected at the voxel level andP < 0.05, FWE corrected at the cluster level)
Anatomical region | Hemisphere | x | y | z | t-score | Voxel size |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Facial aesthetic judgment > facial gender judgment | ||||||
Superior medial frontal cortex | L | −6 | 50 | 43 | 7.762 | 341 |
R | 9 | 47 | 52 | 5.480 | 20 | |
Inferior tempora lgyrus | L | −42 | 8 | −41 | 6.159 | 31 |
Inferior OFC | L | −39 | 29 | −17 | 5.819 | 44 |
Scene Moral Aesthetic Judgment > scene Gender Judgment | ||||||
Inferior OFC | L | −51 | 29 | −2 | 11.601 | 9409 |
R | 45 | 29 | −8 | 11.410 | 172 | |
Superior medial frontal cortex | L | −6 | 50 | 43 | 11.270 | 111 |
ITG | L | −45 | 10 | −41 | 7.570 | 20 |
Cerebellum | R | 24 | −82 | −32 | 11.856 | 899 |
Rectus gyrus | L | 0 | 47 | −20 | 7.016 | 23 |
Middle cingulate | L | −3 | −13 | 37 | 6.566 | 33 |
Middle occipital cortex | R | 34 | 34 | 34 | 5.904 | 45 |
L | −33 | −82 | 31 | 5.553 | 21 | |
Precuneus | R | 9 | −73 | 58 | 5.875 | 53 |
Conjunction analysis | ||||||
Superior medial frontal cortex | L | −6 | 50 | 43 | 7.762 | 340 |
R | 9 | 47 | 52 | 5.480 | 19 | |
ITG | L | −42 | 8 | −41 | 6.159 | 31 |
Inferior OFC | L | −39 | 29 | −17 | 5.819 | 44 |
Note: Thet-scores computed by SPM8 quantify the statistical difference between the two conditions. Coordinates refer to the stereotactic space of the Montreal Neurological Institute.
Critically, as we predicted, the conjunction analysis of the two previous contrasts of interest showed that the neural networks involved in moral aesthetic judgments overlapped with those involved in facial aesthetic judgments (seeTable 1 andFigure 3). To further investigate the neural properties of these key regions, we also conducted an ROI analysis by using a LOOCV procedure. We found that the left inferior OFC, left ITG and bilateral medial SFG were more active during the aesthetic judgment task (P < 0.05) (seeFigure 3). Together, these findings suggest a common neural mechanism underlying aesthetic judgments irrespective of beauty forms, involving the left inferior OFC, left ITG and bilateral medial SFG, providing direct support for our hypothesis.
EXPERIMENT 2
Experiment 2 investigated the network of facial beauty and moral beauty during implicit perception. That is, while Experiment 1 focused on the difference between tasks (aestheticvs non-aesthetic), Experiment 2 focused on the difference between stimuli (beautifulvs common or neutral). Besides, as no aesthetic task was involved, we could examine whether people could appreciate beauty (moral or facial) automatically and implicitly.
Materials and methods
Participants
Twenty-two (mean age = 21, range, 18–25 years; 12 females) healthy right-handed college students who did not participate in Experiment 1 with no history of psychiatric and neurological disorders were recruited as participants. All participants were assessed to ensure normal or corrected to normal vision. Informed consent was obtained from all participants before the experiment according to the requirements of the Ethics Committee of Department of Psychology, South China Normal University.
Stimuli
Facial photographs and scene drawings were used as stimuli. For facial stimuli, we used the same stimulus sets used previously as ‘beautiful’ and ‘common’ faces in Experiment 1 (seeFigure 4). To reduce the visual differences of moral beauty and non-moral beauty scenes, paired scene drawings were created. In each pair, moral beauty and moral neutral scenes had identical backgrounds and characters, and only differed in the character’s actions. Before the experiment, an independent group of 36 participants rated the visual complexity, familiarity, artistry and degree of moral beauty of the candidate stimuli on a 7-point scale. Thirty-six pairs with significant differences in the moral beauty score but not in other indices were thus selected as stimuli for the purpose of achieving a better match of visual processing workload and subjective artistic preference (complexity:t35 = −1.33,P > 0.05; familiarity:t35 = −0.42,P > 0.05; artistryt35 = 1.79,P > 0.05; moral beauty:t35 = 22.73,P < 0.05), forming 36 paired drawings of moral beauty scenes and moral neutral scenes, respectively (seeFigure 4).
Fig. 4.
Examples of stimuli in Experiment 2. Figure (A) and (B) demonstrate two examples of scene drawings, which are matched for visual complexity, familiarity and artistry with identical main character and background. Figure (A) depicts moral beauty scenes, in which the main character is performing a morally beautiful behaviour, and Figure (B) demonstrates morally neutral scene, in which the main character is performing a neutral behaviour. Figure (C) is an example of beautiful face, and Figure (D) is an example of common face.
Task
The same gender judgment task, the details of which were described previously in Experiment 1, was used as the only task of Experiment 2 to keep the participant’s attention fixated on the presented stimuli in the absence of the explicit aesthetic judgment task.
Procedure
We used a blocked design with 12 blocks pertaining to each of the four experimental conditions (FB and FC, SB and SN). Block orders were fixed and counterbalanced across participants. Each block lasted for 15 s and was followed by 10 s of fixation (seeFigure 5). Each block included six trials. Within each block, every image was presented for 2 s subsequently after a 500 ms fixation baseline. Participants were instructed to fixate on each image and make judgments, accordingly, using their thumbs on a button box. Each participant had a single functional scan, which lasted ∼25 min. After the scan, all participants rated the facial photos based on the degree of facial beauty and the scene drawings based on the degree of moral beauty.
Fig. 5.
Experimental design and procedure in Experiment 2. Participants viewed stimuli in four different types of blocks: FB, FC, SB and SN. The neural substrates of facial beauty were identified by the contrast of ‘FB-FA’. The neural substrates of moral beauty were identified by the contrast of ‘SB-SN’. Each block lasted for 15 s and was followed by 10 s of fixation.
fMRI data acquisition and analysis
The fMRI data acquisition and pre-processing were the same as in Experiment 1. At the single participant level, we modelled four regressors of interest: beautiful face (FB), common face (FC), morally beautiful scene (SB), and neutral scene (SN). The four block types were time-locked to the onset of the block with a block duration of 15 s. Six motion parameters estimated during the realignment procedure were included in the model as covariates of no interest. At the first level, analyses were performed individually for each participant and contrast images were subsequently entered into a second-level analysis treating participants as a random factor.
At the group level, analyses were conducted at a voxel threshold of uncorrectedP < 0.001 and a cluster threshold ofP < 0.05 (FWE corrected). We first performed the contrasts of ‘FB > FC’ and ‘SB > SN’, respectively, using the flexible factorial analysis in SPM8. Based on our main interest on the common brain regions involved in the above two contrasts, we then computed a conjunction between the ‘FB > FC’ and ‘SB > SN’ using the minimum statistic approach advocated byNicholset al. (2005). Given the heavy involvement of OFC in mediating beauty in previous studies, we used small volume correction (SVC) on OFC with 10 mm radius centring on the coordinate of the OFC peak voxel (x = −21,y = 54,z = −9) from a previous study (Winstonet al., 2007). Parameter estimates in each ROI were obtained by a LOOCV analysis (Kriegeskorteet al., 2009). This procedure was repeated 22 times in Experiment 2 by omitting a different participant each time, and the parameter estimates were then averaged across participants and plotted.
Moreover, to further investigate the role of cortical reward region (OFC) and subcortical reward region (putamen) in the cerebral representation of beauty, we used the LOOCV procedure to obtain parameter estimates from OFC and putamen across participants, and then conducted a Pearson correlation analysis to examine the relationship between these parameter estimates and the behavioural ratings of facial and moral beauty obtained at the post-scan session. This procedure ensures that we are not prone to a ‘double dipping’ error in the subsequent correlation analyses (Kriegeskorteet al., 2009). Finally, we examined the difference between correlation coefficients by using the Fisher’s test (Fisher and Bennett, 1973).
Results
Behavioral results
Behavioral results in Experiment 2 did not exhibit significant performance differences between beautiful and common faces (RTs:t21 = 0.11, MSE = 9.18,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = 0.01; accuracy:t21 = −0.48, MSE = 0.01,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = −0.11), or moral beauty and moral neutral scenes (RTs:t21 = 0.45, MSE = 10.51,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = 0.04; accuracy:t21 = −1.79, MSE = 0.01,P > 0.05, Cohen’sd = −0.32). However, for the post-scan rating session, we found significant differences between the ratings of beautiful (5.08 ± 0.46) and common (2.97 ± 0.52) faces (t21 = 11.72, MSE = 0.20,P < 0.05, Cohen’sd = 3.87) as well as significant difference between ratings of moral beauty (4.84 ± 0.64) and neutral (3.58 ± 0.49) scenes (t21 = 12.85, MSE = 0.21,P < 0.05, Cohen’sd = 3.07), indicating that there was no mismatch between our stimuli selection criteria and participants’ perception of facial and moral beauty.
Imaging results
We found that compared with FCs, FBs elicited stronger activation in the left inferior OFC, left putamen, left SFG and right middle occipital cortex (seeTable 2 andSupplementary Figure S2). Stronger brain activation related to scenes conveying moral beauty (contrasted with those related to SNs) was observed in a large-scale brain network, including the left inferior OFC, right medial OFC, left middle temporal gyrus, left middle frontal gyrus, angular gyrus, right paracentral lobule, bilateral middle cingulate, left post cingulate, and left precuneus (seeTable 2 andSupplementary Figure S2).
Table 2.
Peak voxel size for all regions obtained from the whole-brain random-effects contrasts in Experiment 2 (P < 0.001, uncorrected at the voxel level andP < 0.05, FWE corrected at the cluster level)
Anatomical region | Hemisphere | x | y | z | t-Score | Voxel size |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Beautiful Face > common Face | ||||||
Putamen | L | −28 | −8 | 10 | 5.18 | 1743 |
Inferior OFC | L | −38 | 42 | −10 | 4.80 | 837 |
SFG | L | −16 | 30 | 44 | 4.44 | 408 |
Middle occipital cortex | R | 38 | −72 | 18 | 4.38 | 458 |
Moral beauty scenes > ordinary scenes | ||||||
Middle temporal gyrus | L | −64 | −16 | −18 | 6.94 | 589 |
Middle frontal gyrus | L | −40 | 18 | 48 | 5.03 | 379 |
Paracentral lobule | R | 4 | −36 | 70 | 5.01 | 2444 |
Middle cingulate | L | −4 | −16 | 40 | 4.69 | 292 |
R | 4 | −22 | 40 | 4.37 | 81 | |
Post cingulate | L | 0 | −36 | 28 | 4.15 | 200 |
Precuneus | L | −14 | −60 | 36 | 4.03 | 81 |
Medial OFC | R | 12 | 52 | −10 | 4.81 | 875 |
Angular | L | −42 | −58 | 38 | 4.76 | 748 |
R | 48 | −60 | 52 | 3.89 | 502 | |
Inferior OFC | L | −36 | 40 | −14 | 4.72 | 651 |
Conjunction | ||||||
Inferior OFCa | L | −38 | 44 | −14 | 4.55 | 101 |
Note: Thet-scores computed by SPM8 quantify the statistical difference between the two conditions. Coordinates refer to the stereotactic space of the Montreal Neurological Institute.
aRefers to the fact that the activation was further corrected by a SVC analysis.
A conjunction analysis of the above two contrasts showed that the left inferior OFC was activated by both facial beauty and moral beauty (seeTable 2 andFigure 6). Given the heavy involvement of the OFC in mediating beauty in previous studies, we corrected for the multiple comparison error rate by applying an SVC on the statistical results (see ‘Materials and Methods’ section). The ROI was defined as a 20 mm sphere centred on the peak voxel in the OFC (x = −21,y = 54,z = −9), identified from a previous study (Winstonet al., 2007). As a result, we discovered significant activation of the OFC (x = −36,y = 38,z = −14, peakt = 4.58) in the conjunction analysis (SVC,P < 0.05, FWE corrected). So, while the conjunction analysis revealed that there is a common cortical reward region (OFC) involved in the implicit perception of both facial and moral beauty, interestingly, the facial and moral beauty analyses revealed that the implicit perception of facial beauty involves both cortical (OFC) and subcortical (putamen) reward regions, whereas the implicit perception of moral beauty involves only a cortical reward region (OFC).
To further understand the neural properties of the OFC and the putamen in relation to facial and moral beauty, we conducted an ROI-based correlation analysis (see ‘Materials and Methods’ section for details). Both OFC and putamen activity positively correlated with facial beauty ratings (OFC:r21 = 0.49,P < 0.05; putamen:r21 = 0.44,P < 0.05) (seeFigure 7A and B). Fisher’s test (Fisher and Bennett, 1973) showed that there were no significant difference between the above correlation coefficients,z = 0.15,P > 0.05, indicating similar involvement of both cortical and subcortical reward systems in the perception of facial beauty. Additionally, we found a significant positive correlation between OFC activity and moral beauty ratings (r21 = 0.67,P < 0.05) (seeFigure 7C) but no relationship between putamen activity and moral beauty ratings (r21 = 0.21,P > 0.05) (seeFigure 7D). Fisher’s test revealed a significant difference between the above correlation coefficients,z = 1.79,P < 0.05, suggesting a stronger cortical (i.e. OFC) than subcortical involvement in the perception of moral beauty.
Fig. 7.
The correlation analysis between brain activity and aesthetic ratings. (A) Correlation results of the activation of OFC with the rating difference of ‘beautiful face > common face’ (FB-FC). (B) Correlation results of the activation of putamen with the rating difference of ‘beautiful face > common face’ (FB-FC). (C) Correlation results of the activation of OFC with the rating difference of ‘moral beauty scene > neutral scene’ (SB-SN). (D) Correlation results of the activation of putamen with the rating difference of ‘moral beauty scene > neutral scene’ (SB-SN). *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01.
DISCUSSION
This study aimed to explore whether the cerebral representation of moral beauty is different from the cerebral representation of facial beauty. Experiment 1 investigated the network of moral aesthetic judgments and FA judgments (compared with non-aesthetic gender judgments). That is, Experiment 1 focused on the difference between tasks (aestheticvs non-aesthetic). We identified the common involvement of the bilateral medial SFG, left ITG and left inferior OFC in moral aesthetic judgments and facial aesthetic judgments. Experiment 2 examined the network of facial beauty and moral beauty during implicit perception. That is, Experiment 2 focused on the difference between stimuli (beautifulvs common or neutral). Besides, as no aesthetic task was involved, we could examine whether people could appreciate beauty (moral or facial) automatically and implicitly. We found that implicit perception of beauty also involved the activity of reward system. Facial beauty involved both the cortical reward region OFC and the subcortical reward region putamen, whereas moral beauty involved only the OFC. Moreover, compared with facial beauty, moral beauty spanned a larger-scale cortical network, indicating more advanced and complex cerebral representations characterizing moral beauty.
The network of moral aesthetic judgments and facial aesthetic judgments
Our finding of the bilateral medial SFG, left ITG and left inferior OFC activation in aesthetic judgment tasks was consistent with previous studies (Mollet al., 2002,2007;Greeneet al., 2004;Heekerenet al., 2005;Jacobsenet al., 2006;Cupchiket al., 2009;Di Dioet al., 2011;Avramet al., 2012). It has been established that the ITG plays a critical part in recognition and representation of visual objects (Ungerleider, 1982;Nobreet al., 1994;Vandenbergheet al., 1996). The SFG is relevant for social cognition (Mollet al., 2002,2007;Greeneet al., 2004;Heekerenet al., 2005;Jacobsenet al., 2006), and the OFC is known for its central role in emotional processing (Camilleet al., 2004;Kringelbach, 2005). Hence, the involvement of these regions in the aesthetic judgments is also in agreement with aesthetic theories, which pose that aesthetic judgment consists of perceptual, cognitive and emotional processing (Berlyne, 1971;Cupchik, 2002;Di Dioet al., 2011). As we hypothesized, our results clearly indicated that the aesthetic judgments, moral and facial alike, commonly relied on the orchestration of perceptual, cognitive and emotional components.
Notably, in addition to the overlapping regions activated during both tasks, moral aesthetic judgments also recruited several cortical regions, including the precuneus, rectus gyrus, middle cingulate and middle occipital lobe (seeTable 1,Supplementary Table S1 andSupplementary Material). Previous studies have demonstrated that precuneus and middle cingulate are related to theory of mind and empathy (Mollet al., 2002,2005;Avramet al., 2012), whereas the rectus gyrus is crucial for self-reference (Baiet al., 2012). Therefore, it is likely that activation in these additional regions served as the extra neural resources recruited for more advanced and comprehensive mental processing of moral aesthetic judgments. It is noteworthy that the explanations of the above activated brain areas are based on findings from previous studies. Further research is needed to explore these possibilities in greater detail.
The network of facial beauty and moral beauty
Our results showed that, similar to earlier findings, highly selective activity in response to beautiful faces was observed in the bilateral inferior OFC and putamen, even during implicit perception (Kranz and Ishai, 2006;Bray and O'Doherty, 2007), indicating that facial beauty is simultaneously mediated by both cortical (OFC) and subcortical (putamen) mechanisms. Furthermore, OFC activity was tuned by moral beauty, as evidently manifested in our ROI-based correlation analysis results. Furthermore, enhanced activity was discovered in a large-scale neural circuit including the left inferior OFC, right medial OFC, left middle temporal gyrus, left middle frontal gyrus, left post cingulate, bilateral middle cingulate and bilateral angular gyrus when contrasting the activation induced by moral beauty scenes to that induced by neutral scenes (seeTable 2,Supplementary Table S2 andSupplementary Material). Previous studies have identified that the middle frontal gyrus is responsible for the memory storage (Leunget al., 2002). Angular gyrus, post and middle cingulate are evidenced to be involved in theory of mind and moral cognition (Zaidel and Nadal, 2011;Young and Dungan, 2012). The middle temporal gyrus is an important part in the neural representation of motor knowledge (Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2000;Ruby and Decety, 2001;Phillipset al., 2002;Hauket al., 2004;Wallentinet al., 2011). To summarize, the recruitment of regions lents direct support to our hypothesis of a more advanced and complex neural representation of moral beauty that spanned from regions responsible for basic perceptual processing to regions executing higher-order functionalities, even during implicit perception. However, further research is needed to directly explore these ideas in greater detail.
Critically, regions exhibiting preference for moral beauty did not involve any part of striatum (Figure 7). Both OFC and striatum are important constituents of the reward circuit (Wise, 2002). The striatum is known for its vital contribution to the cerebral representation of the primary reinforcement that directly satisfies basic physiological needs (Wise, 2002;Pelchatet al., 2004;Grabenhorstet al., 2010;Haber and Knutson, 2010), while a number of studies have identified the significant role of OFC in mediating the mental pleasure related not only to the primary reinforcement, but also to the secondary reinforcement with social implications (Kriegeskorteet al., 2009;Brownet al., 2011;Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011). Whereas previous studies have interpreted the preference of the OFC and putamen to processing beautiful faces as being related to both the social and physiological needs of humans, the subcortical insensitivity coupled with the OFC sensitivity to moral beauty scenes might entail intrinsic, exclusive association between moral beauty and high-level, non-physiological needs of humans, as implicated in the perspective of aesthetic theorists that moral beauty emerges on condition that individuals have internalized social norms and social expectations (Haidt, 2003a;Keltner and Haidt, 2003;Haidtet al., 2004).
In conclusion, this study suggests that moral aesthetic judgments and facial aesthetic judgments both involved the joint participation of perceptual, emotional and cognitive components. However, for the neural substrates of beauty, we found that implicit perception of beauty also involved the reward systems. The overlapping but different reliance on the reward systems implies that facial beauty is associated with both physiological and social needs, whereas moral beauty is associated with merely social needs. Moreover, moral beauty spanned a much larger-scale cortical network, indicating more advanced and complex cerebral representations characterizing moral beauty. Our work thus provides novel and compelling neural evidence in relation to the essence of moral beauty and advances the current knowledge of the neural mechanisms underlying aesthetic processing.
SUPPLEMENTARY DATA
Supplementary data are available atSCAN online.
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The contributions of the first and the second author to this paper are equal.
The authors thank Professor Qi Chen and Professor Hua Jin for their technical support and writing suggestions. This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number 31170997), the Major State Basic Research Development Program of China (grant number 2012CB720700), and the China Scholarship Council.
REFERENCES
- Agnati LF, Agnati A, Mora F, Fuxe K. Does the human brain have unique genetically determined networks coding logical and ethical principles and aesthetics? From plato to novel mirror networks. Brain Research Reviews. 2007;55:68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aharon I, Etcoff N, Ariely D, Chabris CF, O'Connor E, Breiter HC. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron. 2001;32:537–51. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00491-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Algoe SB, Haidt J. Witnessing excellence in action: the ‘other-praising’emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2009;4:105–27. doi: 10.1080/17439760802650519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avram M, Gutyrchik E, Bao Y, Pöppel E, Reiser M, Blautzik J. Neurofunctional correlates of aesthetic and moral judgments. Neuroscience Letters. 2012;534:128–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.11.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bai F, Shi Y, Yuan Y, et al. Altered self-referential network in resting-state amnestic type mild cognitive impairment. Cortex. 2012;48:604–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlyne DE. Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York: Appleton Century Crofts; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:11818–23. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Bermudez P, Evans AC. Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:382–7. doi: 10.1038/7299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bray S, O'Doherty J. Neural coding of reward-prediction error signals during classical conditioning with attractive faces. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:3036–45. doi: 10.1152/jn.01211.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown S, Gao X, Tisdelle L, Eickhoff SB, Liotti M. Naturalizing aesthetics: brain areas for aesthetic appraisal across sensory modalities. Neuroimage. 2011;58:250–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvo-Merino B, Jola C, Glaser DE, Haggard P. Towards a sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art. Consciousness and Cognition. 2008;17:911–22. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camille N, Coricelli G, Sallet J, Pradat-Diehl P, Duhamel J-R, Sirigu A. The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science. 2004;304:1167–70. doi: 10.1126/science.1094550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cela-Conde CJ, Marty G, Maestu F, et al. Activation of the prefrontal cortex in the human visual aesthetic perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101:6321–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401427101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman HA, Kim DA, Susskind JM, Anderson AK. In bad taste: evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science. 2009;323:1222–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1165565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chatterjee A, Thomas A, Smith SE, Aguirre GK. The neural response to facial attractiveness. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:135–43. doi: 10.1037/a0014430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cupchik GC. The evolution of psychical distance as an aesthetic concept. Culture and Psychology. 2002;8:155–87. [Google Scholar]
- Cupchik GC, Vartanian O, Crawley A, Mikulis DJ. Viewing artworks: contributions of cognitive control and perceptual facilitation to aesthetic experience. Brain and Cognition. 2009;70:84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Dio C, Canessa N, Cappa SF, Rizzolatti G. Specificity of esthetic experience for artworks: an FMRI study. Frontiers in human Neuroscience. 2011;5:139. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diessner R, Rust T, Solom RC, Frost N, Parsons L. Beauty and hope: a moral beauty intervention. Journal of Moral Education. 2006;35:301–17. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher RA, Bennett JH. Statistical methods, Experimental Design, and Scientific Inference. London: Oxford University Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Grabenhorst F, D'Souza AA, Parris BA, Rolls ET, Passingham RE. A common neural scale for the subjective pleasantness of different primary rewards. Neuroimage. 2010;51:1265–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron. 2004;44:389–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene JD, Paxton JM. Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:12506–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900152106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science. 2001;293:2105–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1062872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gu Y. Aesthetic Principle. Nanjing: Southeast University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haidt J. Elevation and the positive psychology of morality. In: Keyes CLM, Haidt J, editors. Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-Lived. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003a. pp. 275–89. [Google Scholar]
- Haidt J. The moral emotions. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003b. pp. 852–70. [Google Scholar]
- Haidt J. The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science. 2007;316:998–1002. doi: 10.1126/science.1137651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haidt J, Keltner D. Appreciation of beauty and excellence (Awe, wonder, elevation) In: Peterson C, Seligman MEP, editors. Character Strengths and Virtues: A Handbook of Classification. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 537–52. [Google Scholar]
- Hauk O, Johnsrude I, Pulvermüller F. Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron. 2004;41:301–7. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00838-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heekeren HR, Wartenburger I, Schmidt H, Prehn K, Schwintowski HP, Villringer A. Influence of bodily harm on neural correlates of semantic and moral decision-making. Neuroimage. 2005;24:887–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ishizu T, Zeki S. Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS One. 2011;6:e21852. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobsen T, Schubotz RI, Hofel L, Cramon DY. Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. Neuroimage. 2006;29:276–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James W. The Principles of Psychology. 2011. Lawrence: Digireads.com. [Google Scholar]
- Kawabata H, Zeki S. Neural correlates of beauty. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2004;91:1699–705. doi: 10.1152/jn.00696.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion. 2003;17:297–314. doi: 10.1080/02699930302297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12:48–55. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kranz F, Ishai A. Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Current Biology. 2006;16:63–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PS, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:535–40. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung H-C, Gore J, Goldman-Rakic PS. Sustained mnemonic response in the human middle frontal gyrus during on-line storage of spatial memoranda. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:659–71. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Levitin DJ. The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. Neuroimage. 2005;28:175–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moll J, de Oliveira-Souza R, Eslinger PJ, et al. The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:2730–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02730.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moll J, Oliveira-Souza RD, Garrido GJ, et al. The self as a moral agent: linking the neural bases of social agency and moral sensitivity. Social Neuroscience. 2007;2:336–52. doi: 10.1080/17470910701392024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moll J, Zahn R, de Oliveira-Souza R, Krueger F, Grafman J. The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:799–809. doi: 10.1038/nrn1768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline J-B. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage. 2005;25:653–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nobre AC, Allison T, McCarthy G. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe. Nature. 1994;372:260–3. doi: 10.1038/372260a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelchat ML, Johnson A, Chan R, Valdez J, Ragland JD. Images of desire: food-craving activation during fMRI. Neuroimage. 2004;23:1486–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips JA, Noppeney U, Humphreys GW, Price CJ. Can segregation within the semantic system account for category-specific deficits? Brain. 2002;125:2067–80. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruby P, Decety J. Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:546–50. doi: 10.1038/87510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsukiura T, Cabeza R. Shared brain activity for aesthetic and moral judgments: implications for the beauty-is-good stereotype. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2011;6:138–48. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M. Two cortical visual systems. In: Ingle DJ, Goodale MA, Mansfield RJW, editors. Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1982. pp. 549–86. [Google Scholar]
- Vandenberghe R, Price C, Wise R, Josephs O, Frackowiak R. Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature. 1996;383:254–6. doi: 10.1038/383254a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vartanian O, Goel V. Neuroanatomical correlates of aesthetic preference for paintings. Neuroreport. 2004;15:893–7. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200404090-00032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vessel EA, Starr GG, Rubin N. The brain on art: intense aesthetic experience activates the default mode network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:66. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallentin M, Nielsen AH, Vuust P, Dohn A, Roepstorff A, Lund TE. BOLD response to motion verbs in left posterior middle temporal gyrus during story comprehension. Brain and Language. 2011;119:221–5. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winston JS, O'Doherty J, Kilner JM, Perrett DI, Dolan RJ. Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:195–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA. Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensed incentives. Neuron. 2002;36:229–40. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00965-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young L, Dungan J. Where in the brain is morality? Everywhere and maybe nowhere. Social Neuroscience. 2012;7:1–10. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.569146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaidel D, Nadal M. Brain intersections of aesthetics and morals: perspectives from biology, neuroscience, and evolution. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 2011;54:367–80. doi: 10.1353/pbm.2011.0032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.