Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Jump to content
WikipediaThe Free Encyclopedia
Search

Phonological change

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected fromPhonetic merger)
Phenomenon in phonology
This article has multiple issues. Please helpimprove it or discuss these issues on thetalk page.(Learn how and when to remove these messages)
This articleneeds additional citations forverification. Please helpimprove this article byadding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.
Find sources: "Phonological change" – news ·newspapers ·books ·scholar ·JSTOR
(July 2007) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
This articlepossibly containsoriginal research. Pleaseimprove it byverifying the claims made and addinginline citations. Statements consisting only of original research should be removed.(June 2010) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
This articlemay be too technical for most readers to understand. Pleasehelp improve it tomake it understandable to non-experts, without removing the technical details.(April 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this message)
(Learn how and when to remove this message)
Sound change andalternation
Fortition
Dissimilation
This article containsphonetic transcriptions in theInternational Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, seeHelp:IPA. For the distinction between[ ],/ / and ⟨ ⟩, seeIPA § Brackets and transcription delimiters.

Inhistorical linguistics,phonological change is anysound change that alters the distribution ofphonemes in a language. In other words, a language develops a new system of oppositions among its phonemes. Old contrasts may disappear, new ones may emerge, or they may simply be rearranged.[1] Sound change may be an impetus for changes in the phonological structures of a language (and likewise, phonological change may sway the process of sound change).[1] One process of phonological change isrephonemicization, in which the distribution of phonemes changes by either addition of new phonemes or a reorganization of existing phonemes.[2] Mergers and splits are types of rephonemicization and are discussed further below.

Types

[edit]

In a typological scheme first systematized byHenry M. Hoenigswald in 1965, a historicalsound law can only affect a phonological system in one of three ways:

  • Conditioned merger (which Hoenigswald calls "primary split"), in which some instances of phoneme A become an existing phoneme B; the number of phonemes does not change, only their distribution.
  • Phonemic split (which Hoenigswald calls "secondary split"), in which some instances of A become a new phoneme B; this is phonemic differentiation in which the number of phonemes increases.
  • Unconditioned merger, in which all instances of phonemes A and B become A; this is phonemic reduction, in which the number of phonemes decreases.

This classification does not consider mere changes in pronunciation, that is, phonetic change, evenchain shifts, in which neither the number nor the distribution of phonemes is affected.

Phonetic vs. phonological change

[edit]

Phonetic change can occur without any modification to the phoneme inventory or phonemic correspondences. This change is purelyallophonic or subphonemic. This can entail one of two changes: either the phoneme turns into a new allophone—meaning the phonetic form changes—or the distribution of allophones of the phoneme changes.[2]

For the most part, phonetic changes are examples ofallophonic differentiation or assimilation; i.e., sounds in specific environments acquire new phonetic features or perhaps lose phonetic features they originally had. For example, thedevoicing of the vowels/i/ and/ɯ/ in certain environments inJapanese, thenasalization ofvowels beforenasals (common but not universal), changes inpoint of articulation ofstops and nasals under the influence of adjacent vowels.

Phonetic change in this context refers to the lack of phonological restructuring, not a small degree of sound change. For example,chain shifts such as theGreat Vowel Shift (in which nearly all of the vowels of the English language changed) or the allophonic differentiation of /s/, originally*[s], into[szʃʒʂʐθχχʷh], do not qualify as phonological change as long as all of thephones remain in complementary distribution.

Many phonetic changes provide the raw ingredients for later phonemic innovations. InProto-Italic, for example, intervocalic */s/ became *[z]. It was a phonetic change, merely a mild and superficial complication in the phonological system, but when *[z] merged with */r/, the effect on the phonological system was greater. (The example will be discussed below, underconditioned merger.)

Similarly, in the prehistory ofIndo-Iranian, thevelars */k/ and */g/ acquired distinctivelypalatal articulation before front vowels (*/e/, */i/, */ē/ */ī/), so that*/ke/ came to be pronounced*[t͡ʃe] and*/ge/ *[d͡ʒe], but the phones*[t͡ʃ] and*[d͡ʒ] occurred only in that environment. However, when */e/, */o/, */a/ later fell together as Proto-Indo-Iranian */a/ (and */ē/ */ō/ */ā/ likewise fell together as */ā/), the result was that the allophonic palatal and velar stops now contrasted in identical environments: */ka/ and /ča/, /ga/ and /ǰa/, and so on. The difference became phonemic. (The "law of palatals" is an example of phonemic split.)

Sound changes generally operate for a limited period of time, and once established, new phonemic contrasts rarely remain tied to their ancestral environments. For example,Sanskrit acquired "new" /ki/ and /gi/ sequences viaanalogy and borrowing, and likewise /ču/,/ǰu/, /čm/, and similar novelties; and the reduction of thediphthong */ay/ to Sanskrit /ē/ had no effect at all on preceding velar stops.

Merger

[edit]

Phonemicmerger is a loss of distinction between phonemes. Occasionally, the termreduction refers to phonemic merger. It is not to be confused with the meaning of the word "reduction" in phonetics, such asvowel reduction, but phonetic changes may contribute to phonemic mergers. For example, in mostNorth American English dialects, the vowel in the wordlot and vowel in the wordpalm have become the same sound and thusundergone a merger. In mostdialects of England, the wordsfather andfarther arepronounced the same due to a merger created bynon-rhoticity or "R-dropping".

Conditioned merger

[edit]

Conditioned merger, or primary split, takes place when some, but not all, allophones of a phoneme, say A, merge with some other phoneme, B. The immediate results are these:

  • there are the same number of contrasts as before.
  • there are fewer words with A than before.
  • there are more words of B than before.
  • there is at least one environment for which A, for the time being, no longer occurs, called agap in the distribution of the phoneme.
  • there is, under certain circumstances, analternation between A and B if inflection or derivation result in A sometimes but not always being in the environment in which it merged with B.

Example from Middle English

[edit]
This sectionmay be too technical for most readers to understand. Pleasehelp improve it tomake it understandable to non-experts, without removing the technical details.(April 2021) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

For a simple example, without alternation, early Middle English /d/ after stressed syllables followed by /r/ became /ð/:módor, fæder >mother, father /ðr/,weder >weather, and so on. Since /ð/ was already a structure-point in the language, the innovation resulted merely in more /ð/ and less /d/ and a gap in the distribution of /d/ (though not a very conspicuous one).

Note 1: thanks to borrowing, from dialects as well as other languages, the original distribution has been disturbed:rudder, adder in Standard English (but forms with /ð/ are attested in nonstandard dialects).
Note 2: one who knows German can figure out which cases of English /ð/ were originally /ð/ and which changed from /d/. Original /d/ corresponds to /t/ in German, and original /ð/ corresponds to /d/. Thus,leather = GermanLeder,brother =Bruder,whether =weder,wether =Widder, pointing to original /ð/ in English;weather = GermanWetter,father =Vater,mother =Mutter pointing to original /d/.
Note 3: alternation between /d/ and /ð/ would have been a theoretical possibility in English, as in sets likehard, harder; ride, rider, but any such details have been erased by the commonplace diachronic process calledmorphological leveling.

Devoicing of voiced stops in German

[edit]

A trivial (if all-pervasive) example of conditioned merger is thedevoicing of voiced stops in German when in word-final position or immediately before a compound boundary (see:Help:IPA/Standard German):

  • *hand "hand" > /hant/ (cf. pluralHände /ˈhɛndə/)
  • Handgelenk "wrist" /ˈhantgəlɛŋk/
  • *bund "league, association" > /bʊnt/ (cf. pluralBünde /ˈbʏndə/)
  • *gold "gold" > /gɔlt/
  • *halb "half" > /halp/ (cf.halbieren "to halve" /halˈbiːʁən/)
  • halbamtlich "semi-official" /ˈhalpʔamtlɪç/
  • *berg "mountain" /bɛɐ̯k/ (cf. pluralBerge /ˈbɛɐ̯gə/)
  • *klug "clever, wise" > /kluːk/ (cf. fem.kluge /ˈkluːgə/)

There were, of course, also many cases of original voiceless stops in final position:Bett "bed",bunt "colorful",Stock "(walking) stick, cane". To sum up: there are the same number of structure points as before, /p t k b d g/, but there are more cases of /p t k/ than before and fewer of /b d g/, and there is a gap in the distribution of /b d g/ (they are never found in word-final position or before a compound boundary).

Note 1: this split is easily recoverable byinternal reconstruction because it results inalternations whose conditions are transparent. ThusBund "bunch" (as in, keys) /bʊnt/ has a pluralBünde /ˈbʏndə/ in contrast tobunt "colorful" with /t/ in all environments (feminine /ˈbʊntə/, neuter /ˈbʊntəs/ and so on). In aneutralizing environment, such as a voiceless stop in word-final position, one cannot tell which of two possibilities was the original sound. The choice is resolved if the corresponding segment can be found in a non-neutralizing position, as when a suffix follows. Accordingly, a non-inflected form likeund /ʔʊnt/ "and" is historically opaque (though as the spelling hints, the /t/ was originally *d).
Note 2: unlike most phonological changes, this one became a "surface" rule in German, so loan-words whose source had a voiced stop in the devoicing environment are taken into German with a voiceless one instead:Klub "club" (association) /klʊp/ from Englishclub. The same goes for truncated forms:Bub (for formalBube "boy") is /buːp/.
Note 2a: the surface alternation is what allows modern German orthography to write stops morphophonemically, thusLeib "loaf",Hand "hand",Weg "way", all with voiceless final stops in the simplex form and in compounds, but /b d g/ in inflected forms. In Old High and Middle High German, all voiceless stops were written as pronounced:hleip, hant, uuec and so on.
Note 3: the same distribution holds for /s/ vs. /z/, but it arose by a completely different process, thevoicing of original */s/ between vowels: *mūs "mouse" >Maus /maʊs/, *mūsīz "mice" (for earlier *mūsiz) >Mäuse /ˈmɔʏzə/. Original long (now short)ss does not voice medially, as in küssen "to kiss" /ˈkʏsen/, nor does /s/ from Proto-West-Germanic *t, as inWasser "water" /ˈvasɐ/,Fässer "kegs" /ˈfɛsɐ/ plural ofFass /fas/ (= Englishvat), müßig "idle" /ˈmyːsɪç/. German/ʃ/, as inFisch "fish", reflects original *sk (in native words) and does not become voiced in any environment:Fischer "fisherman"/ˈfɪʃɐ/. (German has/ʒ/ only in loanwords:Genie/ʒeˈniː/ "genius",Gage/ˈɡaːʒə/ "salary".)

Rhotacism in Latin

[edit]

More typical of the aftermath of a conditioned merger is the famous case ofrhotacism in Latin (also seen in someSabellian language spoken in the same area): Proto-Italic *s > Latin /r/ between vowels: *gesō "I do, act" > Lat.gerō (but perfectgessi < *ges-s- and participlegestus < *ges-to-, etc., with unchanged *s in all other environments, even in the same paradigm).

This sound law is quite complete and regular, and in its immediate wake there were no examples of /s/ between vowels except for a few words with a special condition (miser "wretched",caesariēs "bushy hair",diser(c)tus "eloquent": that is, rhotacism did not take place when an /r/ followed the *s). However, a new crop of /s/ between vowels soon arose from three sources. (1) a shortening of /ss/ after a diphthong or long vowel:causa "lawsuit" < *kawssā,cāsa "house' < *kāssā,fūsus "poured, melted" < *χewssos. (2)univerbation:nisi (nisī) "unless" < the phrase *ne sei,quasi (quasī) "as if" < the phrase *kʷam sei. (3) borrowings, such asrosa "rose" /rosa/, from a Sabellian source (the word is clearly somehow from Proto-Italic *ruθ- "red" but equally clearly not native Latin), and many words taken from or through Greek (philosophia, basis, casia,Mesopotamia, etc., etc.).

Nasal assimilation and "gn" in Latin

[edit]
This sectionneeds additional citations forverification. Please helpimprove this article byadding citations to reliable sources in this section. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.
Find sources: "Phonological change" – news ·newspapers ·books ·scholar ·JSTOR
(February 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

A particular example of a conditioned merger in Latin is the rule wherebysyllable-finalstops, when followed by anasal consonant,assimilated with it in nasality, while preserving their original point of articulation:

  • *supimos > *supmos >summus "highest"
  • *sabnyom >Samnium "Samnium" (a region in the southern Apennines)
  • *swepnos >somnus "sleep"
  • *atnos >annus "year"

In some cases, the underlying (pre-assimilation) root can be retrieved from related lexical items in the language: e.g.superior "higher";Sabīni "Samnites";sopor "(deep) sleep". For some words, only comparative evidence can help retrieve the original consonant: for example, the etymology ofannus "year" (as *atnos) is revealed by comparison with Gothicaþna "year".

According to this rule of nasal assimilation, the sequences *-g-n and *-k-n would become[ŋn], with avelar nasal[ŋ]:

The sound [ŋ] was not a phoneme of Latin, but anallophone of /g/ before /n/.

The sequence[ŋn] was regularly rendered in the orthography as |gn|.[4] Some epigraphic inscriptions also feature non-standard spellings, e.g. SINNU forsignum "sign, insigne", INGNEM forignem "fire". These are witness to the speakers' hesitancy on how to best transcribe the sound [ŋ] in the sequence[ŋn].

The regular nasal assimilation of Latin can be seen as a form of "merger", insofar as it resulted in the contrast betweenoral stops (p, b,t, d) andnasal stops (m,n) being regularlyneutralized.

Concerning the number of contrasts

[edit]
This sectionneeds additional citations forverification. Please helpimprove this article byadding citations to reliable sources in this section. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.
Find sources: "Phonological change" – news ·newspapers ·books ·scholar ·JSTOR
(February 2018) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

One of the traits of conditioned merger, as outlined above, is that the total number of contrasts remains the same, but it is possible for such splits toreduce the number of contrasts. It happens if all of the conditioned merger products merge with one or another phoneme.

For example, in Latin, the Pre-Latin phoneme *θ (from Proto-Italic * < PIE *dh) disappears as such by merging with three other sounds: *f (from PIE *bh and *gʷh), *d, and *b:

Initially *θ > f:

  • PItal. *tʰi-n-kʰ- "model, shape" > *θi-n-χ- > Lat.fingō (PIE root *dheyǵh- "smear, work with the hands"; cf. Sanskritdihanti "they smear", Avestan daēza- "wall" = Greekteîkhos; Englishdough < OEdāh besidesdāg < PIE *dhoyǵh-)
  • PItal. *tʰwor- "door" > *θwor- > Lat.forēs "door" (PIE *dhwor-; like most reflexes plural only; cf Eng.door < *dhur-, Greekthúrā (probably < *dhwor-) usuallythúrai pl.)
Cf. Latin ferō "carry" < Proto-Italic *pʰer- < PIE *bher-; Latin frāter "brother" < Proto-Italic *pʰrātēr < PIE *bhre-H₂ter-

Medially adjacent to *l, *r, or *u, *θ becomesb:

  • PItal. *wertʰom "word" > *werθom > *werðom (? *werβom) > Lat.verbum (cf. Englishword < *wurdaⁿ < PIE *wṛdhom, Lithuanianvaṙdas "name")
  • PItal. *rutʰros "red" > *ruθros > *ruðros (? *ruβros) > Latinruber (via *rubers < *rubrs < *rubros), cf.rubra fem.rubrum neut.
  • PItal. *-tʰlo-/*-tʰlā- "tool suffix" > Latin -bulum, -bula: PIE *peH₂-dhlo- "nourishment" > PItal. *pā-tʰlo- > *pāθlo- > Latinpābulum; PIE *suH-dhleH₂- "sewing implement" > PItal. *sūtʰlā > *sūθlā > Latinsūbula "cobbler's awl"
Intervocalic Latin -b- is from PIE *bh, *s, and (rarely and problematically) *b: Lat.ambō "both" < PIE *ambh- or *H₂embh- (cf. Greekamphi-); Lat.crābrō "hornet" <*ḱṛHs-ron- (cf. Vedicśīrṣn- "hornet"); Lat.cannabis "hemp" (cf. Old Englishhænep "hemp"). The change of *-sr- to -br- is itself presumably via *-θr- > *-ðr- > *-βr-.

Elsewhere, *θ becomes d:

  • PItal. *metʰyo- "middle" > *meθyo- > Pre-Lat. *meðyo- > Lat.medius (three syllables; PIE *medhyo-, cf. Sanskritmadhya-, Greekmés(s)os < *meth-yo-)
  • PItal. *pʰeytʰ- > *feyθ- > *feyð- > Lat.fīdus "trusting" (cf. Greekpeíthomai "am persuaded", Englishbid "order, ask")
Intervocalic -d- in Latin comes from PIE *d inped- "foot",sīdere "to sit down",cord- "heart"

There is no alternation to give away the historical story, there, viainternal reconstruction; the evidence for these changes is almost entirely from comparative reconstruction. That reconstruction makes it easy to unriddle the story behind the weird forms of the Latin paradigmjubeō "order", jussī perfect,jussus participle. If the root is inherited, it would have to have been PIE *yewdh-.

Unconditioned merger

[edit]

Unconditioned merger, that is, complete loss of a contrast between two or more phonemes, is not very common. Most mergers are conditioned. That is, most apparent mergers of A and B have an environment or two in which A did something else, such as drop or merge with C.[citation needed]

Typical is the unconditioned merger seen in the Celtic conflation of the PIE plain voiced series of stops with the breathy-voiced series: *bh, *dh, *ǵh, *gh are indistinguishable in Celtic etymology from the reflexes of *b *d *ǵ *g. The collapse of the contrast cannot be stated in whole-series terms because the labiovelars do not co-operate. PIE * everywhere falls together with the reflexes of *b and *bh as Proto-Celtic *b, but *gʷh seems to have become PCelt. *, lining up with PCelt. * < PIE *.

Examples

[edit]
  • OE y and ý (short and long high front rounded vowels) fell together withi and í via a simple phonetic unrounding: OE hypp, cynn, cyssan, brycg, fyllan, fýr, mýs, brýd became modernhip, kin, kiss, bridge, fill, fire, mice, bride. There is no way to tell by inspection whether a modern /i ay/ goes back to a rounded or an unrounded vowel. The change is not even reflected in modern spelling since it took place too early to be captured in Middle English Spelling conventions. Of course, current spellings liketype, thyme, psyche, etc., have nothing to do with OEy = /y/.
  • There is a massive, consistent body of evidence that PIE *l and *r merged totally in Proto-Indo-Iranian, as did PIE *e *o *a into Proto-Indo-Iranian *a.
  • The evolution of Romance shows a systematic collection of unconditioned mergers in connection with the loss of Latin vowel length. Latin had ten vowels, five long and five short (i, ī; e, ē; a, ā; and so on). In the variety of Romance underlying Sardo and some other dialects of the islands, the ten vowels simply fell together pairwise: in no way are Latine, ē, say, reflected differently. In Proto-Western-Romance, the ancestor of French, Iberian, Italian north of the Spezia-Rimini line, etc., however, things happened differently: Latin /a ā/ merged totally, as in Sardo, but the other vowels all behaved differently. Upon losing the feature of length, Latin /ī ū/ merged with nothing, but theshort high vowels, front and back, merged with thelong mid vowels: thus, Latin /i ē/ are uniformly reflected as PWRom. * (in the standard Romance notation), and /u ō/ become *. PWRom. * is reflected in French (in open syllables) as /wa/ (spelledoi);voile "sail",foin "hay",doigt "finger",quoi "what", are from Latinvēlum, fēnum, digitus(via *dictu), quid, respectively. There is no way of telling in French which one of the two Latin vowels is the source of any given /wa/.

Another example is provided byJaponic languages.Old Japanese had 8 vowels; it has been reduced to 5 in modernJapanese.

Split

[edit]

In a split (Hoenigswald's "secondary split"), anew contrast arises when allophones of a phoneme cease being in complementary distribution and are therefore necessarily independent structure points, i.e. contrastive.This mostly comes about because of some loss of distinctiveness in the environment of one or more allophones of a phoneme.

A simple example is the rise of the contrast between nasal and oral vowels in French. A full account of this history is complicated by the subsequent changes in the phonetics of the nasal vowels, but the development can be compendiously illustrated via the present-day French phonemes /a/ and /ã/:

  • Step 1: *a > *ã when a nasal immediately follows: *čantu "song" >[tʃãntu] (still phonemically/tʃantu/);
  • Step 2: at some point in the history of French when speakers consistently stopped making an oral closure with the tongue, we had[tʃãt], that is/tʃãt/ (if not/ʃãt/) and finally, with the loss of the final stop, modern French/ʃã/chant "song", distinct from French/ʃa/chat "cat" solely by the contrast between the nasal and the oral articulation of the vowels, and thus with many other forms in which /a/ and/ã/ contrast.
Note 1: the nasalization of a vowel before a nasal is found very widely in the world's languages, but is not at all universal. In modern French, for example, vowels before a nasal are oral. That they used to be nasalized, like the vowels before lost nasals, is indicated by certain phonetic changes not always reflected in the orthography: Fr.femme "woman" /fam/ (with the lowering of[ɛ̃] (nasalized[ɛ]) to *ã prior to denasalization).
Note 2: unusually for a split, the history of the French innovation, even including some changes in vowel cavity features, can be readily inferred byinternal reconstruction. This is because the contrastive feature [nasal] in a vowel system usually has a nasal in its history, which makes for straightforward surmises. There are also clear alternations, as/bɔ̃/ "good" (masc.) vs./bɔn/ (fem.), while such pairs as /fin/ "fine" (fem.) and/fɛ̃/ (masc.) together with derivatives likeraffiné /rafine/ "refined" indicate what happened to nasalized *i.

Phonemic split was a major factor in the creation of the contrast between voiced and voiceless fricatives in English. Originally, to oversimplify a bit, Old English fricatives were voiced between voiced sounds and voiceless elsewhere. Thus /f/ was [f] infisc[fiʃ] "fish",fyllen "to fill" [fyllen],hæft "prisoner",ofþyrsted [ofθyrsted] "athirst",líf "life",wulf "wolf". But in say the dative singular of "life", that islífe, the form was [li:ve] (as in Englishalive, being an old prepositional phraseon lífe); the plural ofwulf, wulfas, was [wulvas], as still seen in wolves. The voiced fricative is typically seen in verbs, too (often with variations in vowel length of diverse sources):gift butgive,shelf butshelve. Such alternations are to be seen even in loan words, asproof vsprove (though not as a rule in borrowed plurals, thusproofs, uses, with voiceless fricatives).

Note 1: unlike the French example, there is no chance of recovering the historical source of the alternations in English between /s θ f/ and /z ð v/ merely through inspection of the modern forms. The conditioning factor (original location of the voiced alternants between vowels, for example) is quite lost and with little reason to even suspect the original state of affairs; and anyway the original distributions have been much disturbed by analogical leveling.Worthy and (in some dialects)greasy have voiced fricative (next to the voiceless ones inworth and grease) but adjectives in -y otherwise do not alternate:bossy, glassy, leafy, earthy, breathy, saucy, etc (cf.glaze, leaves, breathe, and note that even in dialects with /z/ in greasy, the verbto grease always has /s/).
Note 2: the phoneme/ʃ/ does not alternate with/ʒ/ (and never did). In native words,/ʃ/ is from *sk, and either the change of this sequence to/ʃ/ postdated the rearranging of voicing in pre-Old English fricatives, or else it was phonetically long between vowels, originally, much like the/ʃ/ of present-day Italian (pesce "fish" is phonetically[peʃːe]) and long fricatives, just like sequences of fricatives, were always voiceless in Old English, as incyssan "to kiss". The Early Modern English development of/ʃ/ < */sj/, as innation, mission, assure, vastly postdated the period when fricatives became voiced between vowels.
Note 3: a common misstatement of cases like OE /f/ > Modern English /f, v/ is that a "new phoneme" has been created. Not so. A newcontrast has been created.Both NE /f/and /v/ arenew phonemes, differing in phonetic specifications and distribution from OE /f/. Without doubt, one component in this misunderstanding is the orthography. If, instead of speaking of the development of Old English /f/ we said that OE/ɰ/ split into /f/ and /v/, there would presumably be less confused talk of "a" new phoneme arising in the process.

Loss

[edit]

In Hoenigswald's original scheme, loss, the disappearance of a segment, or even of a whole phoneme, was treated as a form of merger, depending on whether the loss was conditioned or unconditioned. The "element" that a vanished segment or phoneme merged with was "zero".

The situation in which a highly inflected language has formations without any affix at all (Latinalter "(the) other", for example) is quite common, but it is the only one (nominative singular masculine:altera nominative singular feminine,alterum accusative singular masculine, etc.) of the 30 forms that make up the paradigm that is not explicitly marked with endings for gender, number, and case.

From a historical perspective, there is no problem sincealter is from *alteros (overtly nominative singular and masculine), with the regular loss of the short vowel after *-r- and the truncation of the resulting word-final cluster *-rs. Descriptively, however, it is problematic to say that the "nominative singular masculine" is signaled by the absence of any affix. It is simpler to viewalter as more than what it looks like, /alterØ/, "marked" for case, number, and gender by an affix, like the other 29 forms in the paradigm. It is merely that the "marker" in question is not a phoneme or sequence of phonemes but the element /Ø/.

Along the way, it is hard to know when to stop positing zeros and whether to regard one zero as different from another. For example, if the zero not-markingcan (as inhe can) as "third person singular" is the same zero that not-marksdeer as "plural", or if are both basically a single morphological placeholder. If it is determined that there is a zero on the end ofdeer inthree deer, it is uncertain whether English adjectives agree with the number of the noun they modify, using the same zero affix. (Deictics do so: comparethis deer, these deer.) In some theories of syntax it is useful to have an overt marker on a singular noun in a sentence such asMy head hurts because the syntactic mechanism needs something explicit to generate the singular suffix on the verb. Thus, all English singular nouns may be marked with yet another zero.

It seems possible to avoid all those issues by considering loss as a separate basic category of phonological change, and leave zero out of it.

As stated above, one can regard loss as both a kind of conditioned merger (when only some expressions of a phoneme are lost) and a disappearance of a whole structure point. The former is much more common than the latter.

  • In Latin are many consonant clusters that lose a member or two such as these:tostus "toasted, dried" < *torstos,multrum "milking stool" < *molktrom,scultus "carved" < *scolptos,cēna "dinner" < *kertsnā,lūna "moon" < *louwksnā ("lantern" or the like).
  • Greek lost all stops from the end of a word (so *kʷit "what" > Greekti, *deḱṃt "ten" >déka, *wanakt "O prince" > ána), but stops generally survive elsewhere. PIE *s drops medially between voiced sounds in Greek but is preserved in final position and in some consonant clusters.
  • Old English [x] (voiceless velar fricative) is everywhere lost as such, but usually leaves traces behind (transphonologization). Infurh "furrow" andmearh "marrow", it vocalizes. It is elided (with varying effects on the preceding vowel, such as lengthening) innight, knight, might, taught, naught, freight, fought, plow (Brit. plough, OEplōh),bought, through, though, slaughter; but /f/ inlaugh, trough, tough, enough (anddaughter can be found inThe Pilgrim's Progress rhyming withafter, and the spellingdafter is actually attested) The /x/ phoneme still exists in some onomatopoeiac words, like "ugh" (note the spelling usesgh, which indicates that when they were coined, there was still some understanding of the phonemic meaning ofgh), "yech" and "chutzpah".
  • /g k/ are lost in English in word-initial position before /n/:gnaw, gnat, knight, know. /t/ is lost after fricatives before nasals and /l/:soften, castle, bristle, chestnut, Christmas, hasten
  • In many words, /f/ (that is, Old English [v]) was lost between vowels:auger, hawk, newt < OEnafogar, hafoc, efete ("lizard"), and in some alternative (poetic) forms:e'en "evening",o'er "over",e'er "ever"; Scottishsiller "silver", and others.

The ends of words often have sound laws that apply there only, and many such special developments consist of the loss of a segment. The early history and prehistory of English has seen several waves of loss of elements, vowels and consonants alike, from the ends of words, first in Proto-Germanic, then to Proto-West-Germanic, then to Old and Middle and Modern English, shedding bits from the ends of words at every step of the way. There is in Modern English next to nothing left of the elaborate inflectional and derivational apparatus of PIE or of Proto-Germanic because of the successive ablation of the phonemes making up these suffixes.

Total unconditional loss is, as mentioned, not very common. Latin /h/ appears to have been lost everywhere in all varieties of Proto-Romance except Romanian. Proto-Indo-European laryngeals survived as consonants only inAnatolian languages but left plenty of traces of their former presence (seelaryngeal theory).

Phonemic differentiation

[edit]

Phonemic differentiation is the phenomenon of alanguage maximizing the acoustic distance between itsphonemes.

Examples

[edit]

For example, in many languages, includingEnglish, mostfront vowels areunrounded, while mostback vowels are rounded. There are no languages in which all front vowels are rounded and all back vowels are unrounded. The most likely explanation for this[citation needed] is that front vowels have a higher secondformant (F2) than back vowels, and unrounded vowels have a higher F2 than rounded vowels. Thus unrounded front vowels and rounded back vowels have maximally different F2s, enhancing their phonemic differentiation.

Phonemic differentiation can have an effect ondiachronicsound change. Inchain shifts, phonemic differentiation is maintained, while in phonemic mergers it is lost. Phonemic splits involve the creation of two phonemes out of one, which then tend to diverge because of phonemic differentiation.

Chain shifts

[edit]
Main article:Chain shift

In achain shift, one phoneme moves in acoustic space, causing other phonemes to move as well to maintain optimal phonemic differentiation. An example fromAmerican English is theNorthern cities vowel shift[1], where theraising of/æ/ has triggered a fronting of/ɑ/, which in turn has triggered a lowering of/ɔ/, and so forth.

Phonemic mergers

[edit]

If a phoneme moves in acoustic space, but its neighbors do not move in a chain shift, aphonemic merger may occur. In that case, a single phoneme results where an earlier stage of the language had two phonemes (that is also calledphonetic neutralization). A well known example of a phonemic merger in American English is thecot–caught merger by which the vowel phonemes/ɑ/ and/ɔ/ (illustrated by the wordscot andcaught respectively) have merged into a single phoneme in someaccents.

Phonemic splits

[edit]

In a phonemic split, a phoneme at an earlier stage of the language is divided into two phonemes over time. Usually, it happens when a phoneme has twoallophones appearing in different environments, butsound change eliminates the distinction between the two environments. For example, inumlaut in theGermanic languages, theback vowels/u,o/ originally had front rounded allophones[y,ø] before the vowel/i/ in a following syllable. When sound change caused the syllables containing/i/ to be lost, a phonemic split resulted, making/y,ø/ distinct phonemes.

It is sometimes difficult to determine whether a split or a merger has happened if one dialect has two phonemes corresponding to a single phoneme in another dialect;diachronic research is usually required to determine the dialect that is conservative and the one that is innovative.

When phonemic change occurs differently in thestandard language and in dialects, the dialect pronunciation is considered nonstandard and may be stigmatized. Indescriptive linguistics, however, the question of which splits and mergers are prestigious and which are stigmatized is irrelevant. However, such stigmatization can lead tohypercorrection, when the dialect speakers attempt to imitate the standard language but overshoot, as with thefootstrut split, where failing to make the split is stigmatized in Northern England, and speakers of non-splitting accents often try to introduce it into their speech, sometimes resulting in hypercorrections such as pronouncingpudding/pʌdɪŋ/.

Occasionally, speakers of one accent may believe the speakers of another accent to have undergone a merger, when there has really been achain shift.

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^abHenrich Hock, Hans (1991).Principles of Historical Linguistics (Second ed.). Berlin; New York: Walter de Gruyter. pp. 53–4.
  2. ^abCrowley, Terry; Bowern, Claire (2010).An Introduction to Historical Linguistics (4th ed.). Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press. p. 69.ISBN 9780195365542.
  3. ^The same PIE root*H₂egʷnós yielded Greekἀμνόςámnos "lamb". In Latin, PIE labiovelars were regularly delabialized before another consonant:relictus "left behind" < *likʷ-to- (cf.relinquō "leave behind", Greekleipō).
  4. ^While Roman grammarians generally make some fairly fine observations about Latin phonetics, they do not mentiong =[ŋ] despite being thoroughly familiar with the idea from Greek orthography, where |γ| =[ŋ] before /k/ and /g/, as inagkúlos "bent"/aŋkýlos/,ággellos "messenger"/áŋɡellos/. This is likely to be a mere oversight.

Sources

[edit]
  1. Hale, M. (2007), Historical linguistics: Theory and method, Oxford, Blackwell[2][3]
  2. Hale, M., Kissock, M., & Reiss, C. (2014) An I-Language Approach to Phonologization and Lexification. Chapter 20.The Oxford Handbook of Historical Phonology. Edited by Patrick Honeybone and Joseph Salmons
  3. Hoenigswald, H. (1965). Language change and linguistic reconstruction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Phonological_change&oldid=1283408741#Merger"
Categories:
Hidden categories:

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp