Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Jump to content
WikipediaThe Free Encyclopedia
Search

Cotton-top tamarin

This is a good article. Click here for more information.
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Species of New World monkey

Cotton-top tamarin[1]
Male atSingapore Zoo, Singapore
Female atBrevard Zoo, Florida, United States
CITES Appendix I (CITES)[3]
Scientific classificationEdit this classification
Domain:Eukaryota
Kingdom:Animalia
Phylum:Chordata
Class:Mammalia
Order:Primates
Suborder:Haplorhini
Infraorder:Simiiformes
Family:Callitrichidae
Genus:Saguinus
Species:
S. oedipus
Binomial name
Saguinus oedipus
Range of the Cotton-top Tamarin
Synonyms
  • Simia oedipusLinnaeus, 1758

Thecotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) is a smallNew World monkey weighing less than 0.5 kg (1.1 lb). This New World monkey can live up to 24 years, but most of them die by 13 years. One of the smallestprimates, the cotton-toptamarin is easily recognized by the long, whitesagittal crest extending from its forehead to its shoulders. The species is found intropical forest edges andsecondary forests in northwesternColombia, where it isarboreal anddiurnal. Its diet includesinsects and plantexudates, and it is an importantseed disperser in the tropical ecosystem.

The cotton-top tamarin displays a wide variety of social behaviors. In particular, groups form a cleardominance hierarchy where only dominant pairs breed. The female normally gives birth to twins and usespheromones to prevent other females in the group from breeding. These tamarins have been extensively studied for their high level ofcooperative care, as well asaltruistic andspiteful behaviors. Communication between cotton-top tamarins is sophisticated and shows evidence of simplegrammatical structure.

Up to 40,000 cotton-top tamarins are thought to have been caught and exported for use inbiomedical research before 1976, whenCITES gave them the highest level of protection and all international commercial trade was prohibited. Now, the species is at risk due to large-scalehabitat destruction, as the lowland forest in northwestern Colombia where the cotton-top tamarin is found has been reduced to 5% of its previous area. It is currently classified ascritically endangered and is one of the rarest primates in the world, with only 6,000 individuals left in the wild.

Taxonomy and naming

[edit]

S. oedipus has the common names "cotton-top tamarin" and "cotton-headed tamarin" in English. Its name comes from the white hair that spans its head and flows down past the neck.[4] In Spanish, it is commonly calledbichichi, tití pielroja, tití blanco, tití cabeza blanca, ortití leoncito.[2][5] In German-speaking areas, the cotton-top tamarin is commonly known asLisztaffe (literally "Liszt monkey") due to the resemblance of its crest to the hairstyle of Hungarian composer and piano virtuosoFranz Liszt.[6]

The species was first described byCarl Linnaeus in his landmark 175810th edition ofSystema Naturae.[2] asSimia oedipus.[7] Linnaeus chose thespecific nameoedipus, which meansswollen foot, but as the species does not have particularly large feet, it is unknown why he chose this name. (Linnaeus often selected names from mythology without any particular rationale, and he may have used the name ofOedipus, the mythical Greek king of Thebes, more or less arbitrarily.)[8] In 1977,Philip Hershkovitz performed ataxonomic analysis of the species based on fur coloration patterns, cranial andmandibular morphology, and ear size. He classifiedGeoffroy's tamarinS. geoffroyi as a subspecies ofS. oedipus.[9] Subsequent analyses by Hernández-Camacho and Cooper (1976),[10]Russell Mittermeier and Coimbra-Filho (1981),[9] and later Grooves (2001)[11] consider theS. oedipus andS. geoffroyi types to be separate species.

Some researchers, such as Thorington (1976),[9] posit thatS. oedipus is more closely related to thewhite-footed tamarin (S. leucopus) than toS. geoffroyi. This view is supported by Hanihara and Natoria's analysis oftoothcomb dental morphology (1987) and by Skinner (1991), who found similarities betweenS. oedipus andS. leucopus in 16 of 17 morphological traits considered.[9]

This species of white-headed tamarin is thought to have diverged from the otherAmazonian forms such asS. leucopus. This is supported by morphological considerations of the transition from juvenile to adulthood, during which the fur coloration patterns change. significantly and are similar between the two species.[12] Hershkovitz proposed that the separation of the two species happened in thePleistocene at the height of theAtrato River, where it intersected the Cauca-Magdalena. At that time, the area was covered by a sea, which created a geographic barrier that caused the species to diverge through the process ofallopatric speciation. Today, the two species are principally separated by the Atrato River.[12]

Physical characteristics

[edit]

The cotton-top tamarin is part of the most diminutive family of monkeys,Callitrichidae, the marmosets and tamarins; it weighs 432 g (15.2 oz) on average. Its head–body length is 20.8–25.9 cm (8.2–10.2 in), while its tail—which is notprehensile—is slightly longer at around 33–41 cm (13–16 in).[12] The species is notsexually dimorphic, the male and female are of a similar size and weight.[5] Members of the Callitrichinae subfamily (including this species) have sharp nails (tegulae) on all digits except the big toes, which have the flat nails (ungulae) common to otherprimates. Tegulae resemble a squirrel's claws and help with movement through trees.[2][5]

The white hair on the back of the head and neck inspire its common name, "cotton-top".

The cotton-top tamarin has a longsagittal crest, consisting of white hairs, from forehead tonape flowing over the shoulders. The skin of the face is black with gray or white bands located above the eyes. These bands continue along the edge of the face down to the jaw.[13] Tamarins are generally divided into three groups by their facial characteristics: hairy-faced, mottled-faced, and bare-faced. The cotton-top tamarin has fine white hair covering its face, but they are so fine as to appear naked, thus it is considered a bare-faced tamarin. Its lowercanine teeth are longer than itsincisors, creating the appearance oftusks.[14] Like other callitrichids, the cotton-top tamarin has twomolar teeth on each side of its jaw, not three like other New World monkeys.[2]

The cotton-top tamarin has fur covering all of the body except the palms of the hands and feet, the eyelids, the borders of the nostrils, the nipples, the anus, and the penis. The back is brown, and the underparts, arms, and legs are whitish-yellow. Therump and inner thighs and upper tail are reddish-orange. The fur is distributed with varying densities throughout the body: the genital region (scrotum and pubic zone),axilla, and the base of the tail have lower densities, while the forward region is much higher. Many individuals have stripes or whorls of fur of striking coloration on their throats. The cotton-top also has whiskers on its forehead and around its mouth.[12]

Habitat and distribution

[edit]
A sign inTayrona National Natural Park in northern Colombia pointing out the tamarin's endangered status in the only part of the world where it still exists

The cotton-top tamarin is restricted to a small area of northwestColombia, between theCauca andMagdalena Rivers to the south and east, the Atlantic coast to the north, and the Atrato River to the west. They are found exclusively in Colombia; 98% of their habitat has been destroyed. Historically, the entire area was suitable for the cotton-top tamarin, but due to habitat loss through deforestation, it survives in fragmented parks and reserves. One of the most important areas for the cotton-top is theParamillo National Park, which consists of 460,000 hectares (1,800 sq mi) of primary and secondary forests.[2]

The cotton-top tamarin is found in bothprimary andsecondary forests, from humidtropical forests in the south of its range totropical dry forests in the north. It is seldom found at altitudes above 400 m (1,300 ft), but has been encountered up to 1,500 m (4,900 ft).[2][10] It prefers the lower levels of the tropical forests, but may also be found foraging on the ground and between theunderstory and thecanopy. It can adapt to forest fragments and can survive in relatively disturbed habitats. In the dry forests are pronounced seasons. Between December and April, it is dry, while heavy rainfall occurs between August and November which can flood the forest floor. Across its range, annual rainfall varies between 500 and 1,300 mm (20 and 51 in).[5]

Ecology

[edit]

The cotton-top tamarin has a diet of mainly fruit (40%) and animal material (40%).[15] This includes insects, plantexudates such asgum andsap,nectar, and occasionally reptiles and amphibians. Due to its small body size and high food passage rate, its diet must be high-quality and high-energy.Insectivory is common in the cotton-top and the species hunts for insects using a variety of methods: stealth, pouncing, chasing, exploring holes, and turning over leaves.[14]

Tamarins act asseed dispersers in tropical ecosystems. While larger primates eat larger seeds, tamarins eat the smaller ones. The expelled seeds have a highergermination rate than others and ingesting larger seeds may help to dislodge and expel intestinal parasites.[5]

The cotton-top tamarin isdiurnal and sleeps with its social group in trees with foliage cover. The group leaves the sleeping tree together an hour after dawn and spends the day foraging, resting, travelling, and grooming.[5][16] The species is thought to rise late and increases the speed of its foraging and travelling before dusk to avoidcrepuscular andnocturnal predators. Its main predators includeraptors,mustelids,felids, and snakes.[17][18] The cotton-top tamarin is extremely vigilant, always looking for potential predators. When the group is resting, one individual moves apart and acts as a lookout to alert the group if it sees a threat.[5]

The cotton-top tamarin can live as long as 24 years in captivity, while its lifespan in the wild averages 13 years.[5]

Behavior

[edit]
Two cotton-top tamarins feed atUeno Zoo in Japan (video)

Social systems

[edit]

The cotton-top tamarin is a highly social primate that typically lives in groups of two to nine individuals, but may reach up to 13 members.[2][16][19] These small familial groups tend to fluctuate in size and in composition of individuals and a cleardominance hierarchy is always present within a party. At the head of the group is the breeding pair. The male and female in this pair are typically in amonogamous reproductive relationship, and together serve as the group's dominant leaders.[19]

Dominant pairs are the only breeding pair within their groups, and the female generally has authority over the breeding male. While nonbreeding group members can be the leading pair's offspring, immigrant adults may also live with and cooperate in these groups. This social grouping in cotton-top tamarins is hypothesized to arise frompredation pressure.[20] Cotton-top tamarins exhibitprosocial behavior that benefits other members of the group,[21] and are well known for engaging incooperative breeding whereby the group's subordinate adults help in rearing the offspring of the dominant pair. The dominant female is more likely to give birth to non-identical twins than a singleton, so it would be too energetically expensive for just one pair to raise the young.[22]

To prevent younger, subordinate females within the group from breeding, the dominant female usespheromones. This suppresses sexual behavior and delays puberty.[23][24] Unrelated males that join the group can release the females from this reproductive suppression; this may result in more than one female of the group becoming pregnant, but only one of the pregnancies will be successful.[25]

Cooperation

[edit]
A male grooming a female as part of the species' cooperative ritual

In cooperative breeding, the effort put into caring for the dominant breeders' offspring is shared by the group members. Parents, siblings, and immigrant adults share young rearing duties for the breeding pair's young. These duties include carrying, protecting, feeding, comforting, and even engaging in play behavior with the group's young. Cotton-top tamarins display high levels ofparental investment during infant care. Males, particularly those that are paternal, show greater involvement in caregiving than do females.[26] Despite this, both male and female infants prefers contact and proximity to their mothers over their fathers.[26] Males may invest additional support in rearing offspring as a form ofcourtship to win the favor of the group's dominant female. However, evidence indicates that time spent carrying infants does not correlate with a male's overallcopulation frequency.[27]

Since only one female in a group breeds, heavy investment in infant care ensures that all offspring survive until independence. Accordingly, cotton-top tamarins bear excessive costs to care for the group's young.[27][28] Male carriers, especially paternal carriers, incur large energetic costs for the sake of the group's young. This burden may cause some male cotton-tops to lose up to 10–11% of their total body weight.[29] The large weight loss may occur from reduced food intake as infant-carrying inhibitsforaging ability for a carrier. The trend of male-carrier weight loss and decreased food intake is in contrast to the dominant female'speriovulatory period, when she gains weight after increasing her own food intake and relinquishing much of her infant-carrying duties.[28]

Altruism

[edit]

While caregiving by males appears to bealtruistic, particularly in cotton-top sires, the costs of infant care may in fact be tolerated forselfish reasons. Namely, the costs to male weight and foraging ability may, in turn, promote consecutive pregnancies in dominant females, thereby providing more offspring bearing the sire'sgenes.[28] Additionally, the cooperative breeding structure of cotton-tops can change with group size and parental experience. First-time sires spend a greater amount of time carrying the infant than experienced ones, and in smaller groups, sires do a greater proportion of carrying and feeding the infant than in larger groups, where helpers take on more of the work. Total care for infants remains constant with varying group size, and infant outcome is not significantly different in groups that have differing levels of experience in raising offspring.[30]

Once infants reach sufficient age, they permanently leave the backs of their carriers and begin contributing to the group.

Thecooperative breeding hypothesis predicts that cotton-top tamarins engage with this young-rearing paradigm, and in turn, naturally embrace patterns of prosocial behavior.[21] These monkeys engage in such behavior by acting altruistically within their groups in caring for infants, vocalizingalarm calls, and in sharing food. Though some studies indicate that cotton-top tamarins have the psychological capacity to participate in reciprocally mediated altruism,[31] it is unclear whether the cotton-top tamarin acts solely using judgments onreinforcement history.[32]

Other studies involving cotton-top tamarins have hinted that positive reciprocity andreciprocal altruism are irrelevant in the prosociality of these primates.[21] Some researchers believe these primates tend to cooperate for selfish reasons and in situations where they incur some benefit for themselves. That is, cooperation in cotton-top tamarins can be better described bymutualism than by true altruism.[32]

Tamarins in captivity have shown the ability to distinguish other individuals based on cooperative tendencies and past behavior. Cotton-tops ultimately use this information to guide future cooperation. Brief periods ofdefection tend to cause swift, irreparable breakups between these primates and their cooperators. To avoid this, cotton-top tamarins may make economically driven decisions based on the projected incentives of a potential cooperator.[32]

Spite and aggression

[edit]

Despite an expansive array of altruistic behaviors, cotton-top tamarins engage in great bouts ofspite through negative reciprocity and punishment. They have been observed to immediately start denying cooperation with monkeys that deny them benefits.[32] Further, in captivity, these primates are not observed to increase altruistic behavior with fellow primates that are committed fully to cooperation. Based on this, researchers believe that repeated interactions in a cooperative society like that of the cotton-top tamarin can heighten the chances that an individual will designate behavioral punishments to others in its group.[33] This reaction has also been observed in other species.[34] However, these reciprocal punishments, or relative lack of altruistic actions, may alternatively happen as a result of response facilitation that increases the chances of a cotton-top punishing another primate after watching that individual perform a similar action.[35]

Dominant females may evict subordinate females from the group out ofspite.

Another way to look at punishment in cotton-top tamarins is by observing their aggressive behavioral responses within and between groups, as well as between species. The cotton-top tamarin, like many marmosets, other tamarins, and specifically those in thegenusSaguinus, stages aggressive displays almost exclusively towards fellow monkeys that belong to the same gender.[36] These intrasexual displays of aggression are more frequent in females, and are vital when a breeding female is forcing both subadult and adult females to emigrate out of a familial group.[16]

Though aggression can occur within groups, the response towards intruders of another species is much more drastic and can involve asexual dimorphism in displays. Females typically employscent-marking intruder response tactics, whereas males are more prone to vocalizing threats, physical aggression, andpiloerection.[37] Scent-marking in cotton-top tamarins is done in two ways: either usinganogenital scent-marking, orsuprapubic scent-marking. The ability to use both of these separateglandular fields for threat signals may indicate females have developed diverging evolutionary threats through differential use of these markings.[37] These variable signals may be used to sign a territorial encounter, or serve as a reproductive signal. The intensity of female threats is generally comparable when directed at intruders of either gender. In contrast, male cotton-tops are considerably more threatening towards fellow males than towards females.[37]

Communication

[edit]

The cotton-top tamarin vocalizes with bird-like whistles, soft chirping sounds, high-pitched trilling, andstaccato calls. Researchers describe its repertoire of 38 distinct sounds as unusually sophisticated, conforming togrammatical rules. Jayne Cleveland and Charles Snowdon performed an in-depth feature analysis to classify the cotton-top's repertoire of vocalizations in 1982. They concluded that it uses a simple grammar consisting of eight phonetic variations of short,frequency-modulated "chirps"—each representing varying messages—and five longer constant frequency "whistles". They hypothesize that some of these calls demonstrate that the cotton-top tamarin usesphonetic syntax, while other calls may be exemplars oflexical syntax usage.[38] Each type of call is given a letter signifier; for example, C-calls are associated with finding food and D-calls are associated with eating.[39] Further, these calls can be modified to better deliver information relevant to auditory localization in call-recipients.[38] Using this range of vocalizations, the adults may be able to communicate with one another about intention, thought processes, and emotion, including curiosity, fear, dismay, playfulness, warnings, joy, and calls to young.[40]

Language acquisition

[edit]

Over the first 20 weeks, after a cotton-top tamarin is born, it is not fully capable of producing the range of vocalizations that an adult monkey can. Despite this limitation on speech producibility, researchers believe thatlanguage acquisition occurs early on with speech comprehension abilities arising first.[39] Infants can at times produce adult-like chirps, but this is rarely done in the correct context and remains inconsistent across the first 20 weeks of life. Regardless, infant cotton-tops are able to respond in behaviorally appropriate ways to varying contexts when presented with adult chirps. This indicates that verbal perception is a quickly acquired skill for offspring, followed closely by auditory comprehension, and later by proper vocal producibility.[39]

Although limited in their own vocal ranges, juvenile cotton-tops respond appropriately to varying contexts provided by adult vocalizations.

Castro and Snowdon (2000) observed that aside from inconsistent adult-like chirping, cotton-top infants most often produce a prototype chirp that differs in vocalization structure from anything seen in the full adult range of vocalizations. Infants are thought to imitate adult speakers, which use differing calls in various contexts, but by using solely the infant prototypical chirp. For instance, adult cotton-tops are known to significantly reduce the amount of general alarm calling in the presence of infants.[38] This is likely adapted so that adults in close proximity to the groups young do not attract the attention of predators to infant-dense areas. Additionally, infants reduce their prototype chirping in the presence of predators. Whether infants areshadowing the calling behavior of adults or they are comprehending danger remains unclear. However, researchers argue that young cotton-top tamarins are able to representsemantic information regardless of immature speech production.[39]

To confirm the notion that language acquisition occurs as a progression of comprehension before production, Castro and Snowdon (2000) showed that infants respond behaviorally to vocalizing adults in a fashion that indicates they can comprehend auditory inputs. When an adult produces a C-call chirp, used to indicate food preference and when navigating to a food source, an infant approaches the adult caller to be fed, but do not use the prototype calling as a proxy for C-calls.[39] This finding argues for the idea that infants are able to understand vocalizations first, and later acquire the ability to communicate with adult vocalizations.

General calling

[edit]

Among the typical cotton-top tamarin communicative vocalizations, the combination long call (CLC) and the alarm call (AC) are the most heavily represented in the literature. CLCs encompass a range ofcontact calls that are produced by isolated individuals using chirps and whistles.[38][41] This type of call is also used for seemingly altruistic alarm calls, thus adding to its range of cooperative behaviors. It is issued in the presence of kin when a threatening llamas predator is seen. Predators of the cotton-top tamarin includesnakes,ocelots,tayras, and most notably,hawks.[20] Early observations by Patricia Neyman even showed that cotton-tops produce diverse sets of alarm calls that can discriminate the presence of birds of prey versus ground-based predators.[16]

A mature cotton-top tamarin producing vocalization for group mates

CLCs involve the production of complex sequence multisyllabic vocalizations. Researchers have argued that long calls exhibit individual differences, thus can carry information sufficient for recipients to determine caller identity. Usinghabituation-discrimination paradigms in language experiments, this theory has been confirmed multiple times in literature.[41][42] However, the individual syllables within a complete CLC vocalization in isolation of each other do not transfer sufficient information to communicate messages between monkeys. Scientists thus consider the whole, intact string of vocalizations to be the unit of perception for CLCs in the cotton-top tamarin.[43] These examinations may confirm that cotton-tops incorporate a lexical syntax in areas of their communication.

Since tamarins can discriminate between predatory threats using varying vocalizations, recipients of an AC are thought to extract various complex signals from this form of communication. Primarily, cotton-tops are able to glean the identity of the cooperating tamarin through differences in individuals' alarm calls.[20] Further, adults are able to discriminate the gender of callers from their ACs and determine the range of calls within a related tamarin's alarm calling repertoire.[20] Alarm call-based identification is postulated to play a number of functional roles in the cotton-top tamarin. Firstly, an AC recipient is able to identify a cooperating tamarin, and by recognizing which in their group it is, be able to judge the reliability of the AC from past experience. This may arise from a selective pressure for being able to statistically determine the amount of risk present, and how endangered an individual and its group are.[20]

Additionally, being able to localize auditory signals may help determine predator location, especially in the presence of a second AC from a different tamarin in the group. This can help confirm predator presence, type (e.g. flying versus ground-based), and support the recipient intriangulating a predator's location. In the context of the cotton-top's cooperative breeding groups, this is postulated as being adaptive for determining the variable risk to one's group members.[20] For example, a call recipient is able to determine which of its kin are and are not at risk (e.g. young offspring, mates, subordinates, relatives, carriers, etc.) and plan subsequent actions accordingly.

Food calls

[edit]
The species is thought to vocalize food preference using C-calls and food retrieval and eating using D-calls.

The cotton-top tamarin makes selective, specialized vocalizations in the presence of food.[44] These include the C-call, produced when a cotton-top approaches and sorts through food, and the D-call, which is associated with food retrieval and is exhibited while eating.[38]

C-call chirping is believed to be an honest signal for communicating food preference, and a cotton-top tamarin more often and more rapidly vocalizes with these chirps when approaching a highly favored food source. Functionally, this behavior may inform other tamarins of the actions the caller will take in a feeding context and whether a preferable food source is available.[44] Despite this research indicating that food calls may be informative to fellow group mates, other observations of cotton-tops show that quantity and distribution of food and audience do not significantly alter a caller's food-centered vocalizations.[45]

The cotton-top tamarin is seen to produce food calls both in the presence and absence of group members.[45] Additionally, response to food calls are directed back to an original caller independent of visual confirmation of a food source. While this may appear to be a result of a very primitive form of communication, Roush and Snowdon (2005) maintain that the food-calling behavior confers some mentally representable information about food to recipient tamarins.

Conservation status

[edit]

The wild population is estimated at 6,000 individuals, with 2,000 adults.[2] This species is critically endangered, and was listed in "The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates between 2008 and 2012."[46] The publication lists highly endangered primate species and is released every two years by theInternational Union for Conservation of Nature Species Survival Commission Primate Specialist Group. The cotton-top tamarin was not selected for the 2012–2014 publication.[47]

The species is critically endangered, with a wild population of merely 6,000 individuals including about 2,000 free-roaming adults.

Habitat destruction through forest clearing is the main cause of this collapse, and the cotton-top has lost more than three-quarters of its original habitat todeforestation,[2] while the lowland forest in which it lives has been reduced to 5% of its historical range. This land is then used for large-scale agricultural production (i.e. cattle) and farming, logging,oil palm plantations, andhydroelectric projects that fragment the cotton-top tamarin's natural range.[48]

The illegal pet trade and scientific research have also been cited as factors by the IUCN. While biomedical studies have recently limited their use of this species, illegal capture for the pet trade still plays a major role in endangering the cotton-top.[49] Before 1976, whenCITES listed the species underAppendix I banning all international trade, the cotton-top tamarin was exported for use inbiomedical research.

In captivity, the cotton-top is highly prone tocolitis, which is linked to an increased risk of a certain type ofcolon cancer. Up to 40,000 individuals were caught and exported for research into those diseases, as well asEpstein–Barr virus, for the benefit of humans. The species is now protected by international law. Although enough individuals are in captivity to sustain the species, it is still critically endangered in the wild.[48]

TheProyecto Tití ("Project Tamarin")[50] was started in 1985 to provide information and support in conservation of the cotton-top tamarin and its habitat in northern Colombia. Proyecto Tití's programs combine field research, education, and community programs to spread awareness about this endangered species and encourage the public to participate in its protection. It now has partner status with theWildlife Conservation Network.[51]

References

[edit]
  1. ^Groves, C. P. (2005).Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.).Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 135.ISBN 0-801-88221-4.OCLC 62265494.
  2. ^abcdefghijRodríguez, V.; Link, A.; Guzman-Caro, D.; Defler, T.R.; Palacios, E.; Stevenson, P.R.; Mittermeier, R.A. (2021) [amended version of 2020 assessment]."Saguinus oedipus".IUCN Red List of Threatened Species.2021: e.T19823A192551067.doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2021-1.RLTS.T19823A192551067.en.
  3. ^"Appendices | CITES".cites.org. Retrieved2022-01-14.
  4. ^Burton, R. (2002). "Pinché tamarin". In Burton, M. (ed.).Marshall Cavendish International Wildlife Encyclopedia. Vol. 19. Marshall Cavendish. p. 2625.ISBN 978-0-86307-949-8.
  5. ^abcdefghCawthon Lang, K. A. (18 May 2005)."Primate Factsheets: Cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) Taxonomy, Morphology, & Ecology".Primate Info Net. Wisconsin Primate Research Center (WPRC) Library. Retrieved19 November 2012.
  6. ^German Wikipedia article[circular reference]
  7. ^Linnaeus, Carolus (1758).Systema naturae per regna tria naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus, differentiis, synonymis, locis. Tomus I. Editio decima, reformata (in Latin). Holmiae (Laurentii Salvii). p. 28.
  8. ^Beolens, B.; Watkins, M.; Grayson, M. (2009).The Eponym Dictionary of Mammals. Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 296.ISBN 978-0-8018-9533-3.
  9. ^abcdEstrada, A. (2006).New Perspectives in the Study of Mesoamerican Primates: Distribution, Ecology, Behavior, and Conservation. Springer. p. 35.ISBN 0-387-25854-X.
  10. ^abHernández-Camacho, J.; Cooper, R. W. (1976). "The nonhuman primates of Colombia". In Thorington Jr., J. R.; Heltne, P. G. (eds.).Neotropical Primates: Field Studies and Conservation. National Academy of Sciences. pp. 35–69.
  11. ^Defler, T. (2004). Mahecha, J. V. R. (ed.).Primates of Colombia (2nd ed.). CONSERVATION International.ISBN 978-1-881173-83-0.
  12. ^abcdHershkovitz, P. (1977)."Saguinus oedipus Linnaeus, Crested Tamarins: 1. History, Characters, Evolution, and Subspecies; and 2. Biology of Crested Bare-face Tamarins".Living New World Monkeys (Platyrrhini): with an Introduction to Primates. University of Chicago Press. pp. 753–.ISBN 978-0-226-32788-4.
  13. ^Eisenberg, J. F.; Redford, K. H. (1999). "The Contemporary Mammalian Fauna: Order Primates".Mammals of the Neotropics: The Central Neotropics: Ecuador, Peru, Bolivia, Brazil. University of Chicago Press. pp. 230–.ISBN 978-0-226-19542-1.
  14. ^abGarber, P. A. (1993). "Feeding ecology and behaviour of the genusSaguinus". In Rylands, A. B. (ed.).Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. Oxford University Press.ISBN 978-0-19-854022-9.
  15. ^Fleagle, J. G. (1998).Primate Adaptation and Evolution (2nd ed.). Academic Press. p. 165.ISBN 978-0-12-260341-9.
  16. ^abcdNeyman, P. F. (1978). "Aspects of the ecology and social organization of free-ranging cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) and the conservation status of the species". In Kleiman, D. G. (ed.).The Biology and Conservation of the Callitrichidae(PDF). Smithsonian Institution Press. pp. 39–71.ISBN 978-0-87474-587-0.
  17. ^Snowdon, C. T.; Soini, P. (1988). "The tamarins, genusSaguinus". InMittermeier, R. A.; Coimbra-Filho, A. F.; da Fonseca, G. A. B. (eds.).Ecology and Behavior of Neotropical Primates. Vol. 2. World Wildlife Fund. pp. 223–298.
  18. ^Sussman, R. W. (2000).Primate Ecology and Social Structure. Volume 2, New World Monkeys. Pearson Preston Hall. pp. 207–.ISBN 978-0-536-74364-0.
  19. ^abSavage, A.; Giraldo, L. H.; Soto, L. H.; Snowdon, C. T. (1996). "Demography, group composition, and dispersal in wild cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) groups".American Journal of Primatology.38 (1):85–100.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1996)38:1<85::AID-AJP7>3.0.CO;2-P.PMID 31914712.S2CID 84120098.
  20. ^abcdefSproul, C.; Palleroni, A.; Hauser, M. D. (2006). "Cottontop tamarin,Saguinus oedipus, alarm calls contain sufficient information for recognition of individual identity".Animal Behaviour.72 (6): 1379.doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.006.S2CID 53182811.
  21. ^abcCronin, K. A.; Schroeder, K. K. E.; Snowdon, C. T. (2010)."Prosocial behaviour emerges independent of reciprocity in cottontop tamarins".Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.277 (1701):3845–3851.doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0879.hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0012-2865-B.PMC 2992700.PMID 20630886.
  22. ^Savage, A.; Snowdon, C. T.; Giraldo, L. H.; Soto, L. H. (1996). "Parental care patterns and vigilance in wild cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)". In Norconk, M. A.; Rosenberger, A. L.; Garber, P. A. (eds.).Adaptive Radiations of Neotropical Primates. Plenum Press. pp. 187–199.ISBN 978-0-306-45399-1.
  23. ^Savage, A.; Ziegler, T. E.; Snowdon, C. T. (1988). "Sociosexual development, pair bond formation, and mechanisms of fertility suppression in female cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus oedipus)".American Journal of Primatology.14 (4):345–359.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350140404.PMID 31968911.S2CID 85779307.
  24. ^Heistermann, M.; Kleis, E.; Pröve, E.; Wolters, H. J. R. (1989). "Fertility status, dominance, and scent marking behavior of family-housed female cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) in absence of their mothers".American Journal of Primatology.18 (3):177–189.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350180302.PMID 31964031.S2CID 85794285.
  25. ^Savage, A.; Shideler, S. E.; Soto, L. H.; Causado, J.; Giraldo, L. H.; Lasley, B. L.; Snowdon, C. T. (1997). "Reproductive events of wild cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) in Colombia".American Journal of Primatology.43 (4):329–337.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1997)43:4<329::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-Z.PMID 9403097.S2CID 6750233.
  26. ^abCleveland, J.; Snowdon, C. T. (1984). "Social development during the first twenty weeks in the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus o. oedipus)".Animal Behaviour.32 (2): 432.doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80279-1.S2CID 53189140.
  27. ^abTardif, S. D.; Bales, K. (1997). "Is infant-carrying a courtship strategy in callitrichid primates?".Animal Behaviour.53 (5): 1001.doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0353.S2CID 3714267.
  28. ^abcSánchez, S.; Peláez, F.; Gil-Bürmann, C.; Kaumanns, W. (1999). "Costs of infant-carrying in the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus)".American Journal of Primatology.48 (2):99–111.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1999)48:2<99::AID-AJP2>3.0.CO;2-6.PMID 10333430.S2CID 31747571.
  29. ^Achenbach, G. G.; Snowdon, C. T. (2002)."Costs of caregiving: weight loss in captive adult male cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) following the birth of infants".International Journal of Primatology.23 (1):179–189.doi:10.1023/A:1013210226793.PMC 1483063.PMID 16804560.
  30. ^Washabaugh, K. F.; Snowdon, C. T.; Ziegler, T. E. (2002). "Variations in care for cottontop tamarin,Saguinus oedipus, infants as a function of parental experience and group size".Animal Behaviour.63 (6): 1163.doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.3018.S2CID 53167116.
  31. ^Hauser, M. D.; Chen, M. K.; Chen, F.; Chuang, E. (2003)."Give unto others: Genetically unrelated cotton-top tamarin monkeys preferentially give food to those who altruistically give food back".Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.270 (1531):2363–2370.doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2509.PMC 1691522.PMID 14667352.
  32. ^abcdHauser, M.; McAuliffe, K.; Blake, P. R. (2009)."Evolving the ingredients for reciprocity and spite".Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.364 (1533):3255–3266.doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0116.PMC 2781875.PMID 19805432.
  33. ^Clutton-Brock, T. H.; Parker, G. A. (1995). "Punishment in animal societies".Nature.373 (6511):209–216.Bibcode:1995Natur.373..209C.doi:10.1038/373209a0.PMID 7816134.S2CID 21638607.
  34. ^Hammerstein, P. (2003). "Why is reciprocity so rare in social animals? A protestant appeal".The Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. MIT Press. pp. 83–93.ISBN 978-0-262-08326-3.
  35. ^Byrne, R. W. (1994)."The evolution of intelligence". In Slater, P. J. B.; Halliday, T. R. (eds.).Behavior and Evolution. Cambridge University Press. pp. 223–265.ISBN 978-0-521-42923-8.
  36. ^Kleiman, D. G. (1979)."Parent-offspring conflict and sibling competition in a monogamous primate"(PDF).American Naturalist.114 (5):753–760.doi:10.1086/283524.S2CID 55162428.
  37. ^abcFrench, J. A.; Snowdon, C. T. (1981). "Sexual dimorphism in responses to unfamiliar intruders in the tamarin,Saguinus oedipus".Animal Behaviour.29 (3): 822.doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80016-4.S2CID 53151699.
  38. ^abcdeCleveland, J.; Snowdon, C. T. (1982). "The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus)".Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie.58 (3): 231.doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1982.tb00320.x.
  39. ^abcdeCastro, N.; Snowdon, C. (2000). "Development of vocal responses in infant cotton-top tamarins".Behaviour.137 (5): 629.doi:10.1163/156853900502259.
  40. ^"Cotton-top Tamarin". Oakland Zoo. Archived fromthe original on 18 June 2012. Retrieved20 June 2012.
  41. ^abWeiss, D.; Garibaldi, B.; Hauser, M. D. (2001). "The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments".Journal of Comparative Psychology.115 (3):258–271.doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.3.258.PMID 11594495.
  42. ^Weiss, D.; Hauser, M. (2002). "Perception of harmonics in the combination long call of cotton-top tamarins,Saguinus oedipus".Animal Behaviour.64 (3):415–426.doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.3083.S2CID 49480605.
  43. ^Miller, C. T.; Ghazanfar, A. A. (2002)."Meaningful acoustic units in nonhuman primate vocal behavior". In Bekoff, M.; Allen, C.; Burghardt, G. (eds.).Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition. MIT Press. pp. 265–274.ISBN 978-0-262-52322-6.
  44. ^abElowson, A. M.; Tannenbaum, P. L.; Snowdon, C. T. (1991). "Food-associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins".Animal Behaviour.42 (6): 931.doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80145-9.S2CID 53252085.
  45. ^abRoush, R. S.; Snowdon, C. T. (2001). "Food transfer and development of feeding behavior and food-associated vocalizations in cotton-top tamarins".Ethology.107 (5):415–429.Bibcode:2001Ethol.107..415R.doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00670.x.
  46. ^Mittermeier, R. A.; Wallis, J.; Rylands, A. B.; Ganzhorn, J. U.; Oates, J. F.; Williamson, E. A.; Palacios, E.; Heymann, E. W.; Kierulff, M. C. M.; Long Yongcheng; Supriatna, J.; Roos, C.; Walker, S.; Cortés-Ortiz, L.; Schwitzer, C., eds. (2009).Primates in Peril: The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates 2008–2010(PDF). Illustrated by S. D. Nash. IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), and Conservation International (CI). pp. 1–92.ISBN 978-1-934151-34-1.
  47. ^Mittermeier, R. A.; Schwitzer, C.; Rylands, A. B.; Taylor, L. A.; Chiozza, F.; Williamson, E. A.; Wallis, J., eds. (2012)."Primates in Peril: The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates 2012–2014"(PDF). Illustrated by S. D. Nash. IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), and Conservation International (CI). pp. 1–40. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2013-10-21. Retrieved2013-03-04.
  48. ^abCawthon Lang, K. A. (18 May 2005)."Primate Factsheets: Cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) Conservation".Primate Info Net. Wisconsin Primate Research Center (WPRC) Library. Retrieved5 March 2013.
  49. ^Sherlock, Gemma (August 9, 2024)."Rare monkey twins born at Chester Zoo".BBC. RetrievedAugust 9, 2024.
  50. ^"Proyecto Tití". Proyecto Tití. Retrieved29 May 2013.
  51. ^"Cotton-top Tamarin Conservation".Wildlife Conservation Network. Retrieved29 May 2013.
Extant species of familyCallitrichidae
Callithrix
(Atlantic marmosets)
Mico
(Amazonian marmosets)
Cebuella
Leontopithecus
(lion tamarins)
Leontocebus
(saddle-back tamarins)
Saguinus
(tamarins)
SubgenusSaguinus:
SubgenusTamarinus:
Callimico
Saguinus oedipus
Simia oedipus
Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Cotton-top_tamarin&oldid=1273521500"
Categories:
Hidden categories:

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp