They are medium to large in size, with strong feet and bills,rictal bristles, and a singlemoult each year (most passerines moult twice). Corvids are found worldwide, except for the southern tip ofSouth America and the polarice caps.[3] The majority of the species are found in tropical South andCentral America and in southern Asia, with fewer than 10 species each in Africa andAustralasia. The genusCorvus has re-entered Australia in relatively recent geological prehistory, with five species and one subspecies there. Several species of raven have reached oceanic islands, and some of these species are now highly threatened withextinction, or have already become extinct.
The name Corvidae for thefamily was introduced by the English zoologistWilliam Elford Leach in a guide to the contents of theBritish Museum published in 1820.[8][9] Over the years, much disagreement has arisen on the exact evolutionary relationships of the corvid family and their relatives. What eventually seemed clear was that corvids are derived fromAustralasian ancestors,[10] and spread throughout the world from there. Other lineages derived from these ancestors evolved into ecologically diverse, but often Australasian, groups. In the late 1970s and throughout the 1980s,Sibley and Ahlquist united the corvids with other taxa in theCorvida, based onDNA–DNA hybridization. The presumed corvid relatives included:currawongs,birds of paradise,whipbirds,quail-thrushes,whistlers,monarch flycatchers anddrongos,shrikes,vireos, andvangas,[2] but current research favors the theory that this grouping is partly artificial. The corvids constitute the core group of theCorvoidea, together with their closest relatives (the birds of paradise,Australian mud-nesters, and shrikes). They are also the core group of the Corvida, which includes the related groups, such asOld World orioles and vireos.[11]
Clarification of the interrelationships of the corvids has been achieved based oncladistic analysis of severalDNA sequences.[11][12] The jays and magpies do not constitutemonophyletic lineages, but rather seem to split up into anAmerican andOld World lineage, and anHolarctic and Oriental lineage, respectively. These are not closely related among each other. The position of theazure-winged magpie, which has always been of undistinguished lineage, is less clear than previously thought.
Thecrested jayshrike (Platylophus galericulatus) is traditionally included in the Corvidae, but is not a true member of this family, being closer to thehelmetshrikes (Malaconotidae) orshrikes (Laniidae).[1][13] Likewise, theHume's ground "jay" (Pseudopodoces humilis) is, in fact, a member of the tit family,Paridae.[14] The following tree showing thephylogeny of the crow family is based on a molecular study by Jenna McCullough and collaborators published in 2023.[15]
The earliest corvidfossils date to mid-Miocene Europe,[16] about 17 million years ago;Miocorvus andMiopica may be ancestral to crows and some of the magpie lineage, respectively, or similar to the living forms, due toconvergent evolution. The known prehistoric corvid genera appear to be mainly of the New World and Old World jay and Holarctic magpie lineages:
Corvids are large to very largepasserines with a robust build and strong legs; all species, except thepinyon jay, havenostrils covered by bristle-like feathers.[18] Many corvids of temperate zones have mainly black or blue colouredplumage; however, some are pied black and white, some have a blue-purple iridescence, and many tropical species are brightly coloured. The sexes are very similar in color and size. Corvids have strong, stout bills and large wingspans. The family includes the largest members of thepasserine order.
The smallest corvid is thedwarf jay (Cyanolyca nanus), at 41 g (1.4 oz) and 21.5 cm (8.5 in). The largest corvids are thecommon raven (Corvus corax) and thethick-billed raven (Corvus crassirostris), both of which regularly exceed 1,400 grams (3.1 pounds) and 65 cm (26 in).
Species can be identified based on size, shape, and geography; however, some, especially theAustralian crows, are best identified by their raucous calls.[2]
Corvids occur in most climatic zones. Most are sedentary, and do notmigrate significantly. However, during a shortage of food, irruptive migration can occur.[2] When species are migratory, they will form large flocks in the fall (around August in the Northern Hemisphere) and travel south.[19]
One reason for the success of crows, compared to ravens, is their ability to overlap breeding territory. During breeding season, crows were shown to overlap breeding territory six times as much as ravens. This invasion of breeding ranges allowed a related increase in local population density.[20]
Since crows and magpies have benefited and even increased in numbers due to human development, it was suggested that this might cause increased rates of nest predation of smaller bird species, leading to declines. Several studies have shown this concern to be unfounded. One study examinedAmerican crows, which had increased in numbers, were a suspect in nest predation of threatenedmarbled murrelets. However,Steller's jays, which are successful independently of human development, are more efficient in plundering small birds' nests thanAmerican crows andcommon ravens. Therefore, the human relationship with crows and ravens did not significantly increase nest predation when compared to other factors, such ashabitat destruction.[20] Similarly, a study examining the decline of British songbirds found no link betweenEurasian magpie numbers and population changes of 23 songbird species.[21]
Some corvids have strong organization and community groups. Jackdaws, for example, have a strong social hierarchy, and are facultatively colonial during breeding.[22] Providing mutual aid has also been recorded within many of the corvid species.
Young corvids have been known toplay and take part in elaborate socialgames. Documented group games follow "king of the mountain" or "follow the leader" patterns. Other play involves the manipulation, passing, and balancing of sticks. Corvids also take part in other activities, such as sliding down smooth surfaces. These games are understood to play a large role in the adaptive and survival ability of the birds.[23]
Mate selection is quite complex, and accompanied with much social play in the Corvidae. Youngsters of social corvid species undergo a series of tests, including aerobatic feats, before being accepted as a mate by the opposite sex.[19]
Some corvids can be aggressive.Blue jays, for example, are well known to attack anything that threatens their nest. Crows have been known to attack dogs, cats, ravens, and birds of prey. Most of the time, these assaults take place as a distraction long enough to allow an opportunity for stealing food.[19]
Corvids are highly opportunistic foragers. Here, ajungle crow feeds on a shark carcass.
The natural diet of many corvid species is omnivorous, consisting ofinvertebrates, nestlings, small mammals, berries, fruits, seeds, andcarrion. However, some corvids, especially the crows, have adapted well to human conditions, and have come to rely on human food sources. In a US study ofAmerican crows,common ravens, andSteller's jays around campgrounds and human settlements, the crows appeared to have the most diverse diet of all, takinganthropogenic foods, such as: bread, spaghetti, fried potatoes, dog food, sandwiches, and livestock feed. The increase in available human food sources is contributing to population rises in some corvid species.[20]
Some corvids are predators of other birds. During the wintering months, corvids typically form foraging flocks.[2] However, some crows also eat many agricultural pests, includingcutworms,wireworms,grasshoppers, and harmful weeds.[19] Some corvids will eatcarrion, and since they lack a specialized beak for tearing into flesh, they must wait until animals are opened, whether by other predators or as roadkill.
Many species of corvid areterritorial, protecting territories throughout the year, or simply during the breeding season. In some cases, territories may only be guarded during the day, with the pair joining off-territory roosts at night. Some corvids are well-known communal roosters. Some groups of roosting corvids can be very large, with a roost of 65,000rooks counted inScotland.[24] Some, including the rook and thejackdaw, are also communal nesters.
The partner bond in corvids is extremely strong, and even lifelong in some species. This monogamous lifestyle, however, can still contain extra-pair copulations.[25] Males and females build large nests together in trees or on ledges; jackdaws are known to breed in buildings and in rabbitwarrens.[22] The male will also feed the female during incubation.[26] The nests are constructed of a mass of bulky twigs lined with grass and bark. Corvids can lay between 3 and 10 eggs, typically ranging between 4 and 7. The eggs are usually greenish in colour with brown blotches. Once hatched, the young remain in the nests for up to 6–10 weeks depending on the species.
Corvids use several different forms of parental care, includingbi-parental care andcooperative breeding.[27]Cooperative breeding takes place when parents are helped in raising their offspring, usually by relatives, but also sometimes by non-related adults.[28][29] Suchhelpers at the nest in most cooperatively-breeding birds are males, while females join other groups.White-throated magpie-jays are cooperatively-breeding corvids where the helpers are mostly female.
Jerison (1973) has suggested that the degree of brainencephalization (the ratio of brain size to body size, EQ) may correlate with ananimal's intelligence andcognitive skills.[30] Corvids andpsittacids have higher EQ than other bird families, similar to that of the apes. Among the Corvidae, ravens possess the largest brain to body size ratio.[31] In addition to the high EQ, the Corvid's intelligence is boosted by their living environment. Firstly, Corvids are found in some of the harshest environments on Earth, where surviving requires higher intelligence and better adaptations. Secondly, most of the Corvids are omnivorous, suggesting that they are exposed to a wider variety of different stimuli and environments. Furthermore, many corvid species live in a large family group, and demonstrate high social complexities.[32]
Their intelligence is boosted by the long growing period of the young.[33][34][35] By remaining with the parents, the young have more opportunities to learn necessary skills.
When compared to dogs and cats in an experiment testing the ability to seek out food according to three-dimensional clues, corvids out-performed the mammals.[36] Ameta-analysis testing how often birds invented new ways to acquire food in the wild found corvids to be the most innovative birds.[37] A 2004 review suggested that their cognitive abilities are on par with those of non-humangreat apes.[38] Despite structural differences, the brains of corvids and great apes both evolved the ability to make geometrical measurements.
Ravens are found to show bystander affiliation, and solicited bystander affiliation after aggressive conflicts.[39] Most of the time, bystanders already sharing a valuable relationship with the victim are more likely to affiliate with the victim to alleviate the victim's distress ("consolation") as a representation ofempathy. Ravens are believed to be able to be sensitive to other's emotions.
Emotion contagion refers to theemotional state matching between individuals. Adriaense et al. (2018) used abias paradigm to quantifyemotional valence, which along withemotional arousal, define emotions.[40] They manipulated the positive and negative affective states in the demonstrator ravens, which showed significantly different responses to the two states: behaving pessimism to the negative states, and optimism to the positive states. Then, the researchers trained another observer raven to first observe the demonstrator's responses. The observer raven was then presented with ambiguous stimuli. The experiment results confirmed the existence of negative emotional contagions in ravens, while the positive emotional contagion remained unclear. Therefore, ravens are capable of both discerning the negative emotions in their conspecifics and showing signs of empathy.[41]
Interspecific communications are evolutionarily beneficial for species living in the same environment. Facial expressions are the most widely used method to express emotions by humans. Tate et al. (2006) explored the issue of non-human mammals processing the visual cues from faces to achieve interspecific communication with humans.[42] Researchers also examined the avian species' capabilities to interpret this non-verbal communication, and their extent of sensitivity to human emotions. Based on the experimental subject of American Crows' behavioral changes to varying human gazes and facial expressions, Clucas et al. (2013) identified that crows are able to change their behaviors to the presence of direct human gaze, but did not respond differentially to human emotional facial expressions. They further suggested that the high intelligence of the crows enables them to adapt well to human-dominated environments.[43]
It is considered difficult to study emotions in animals when humans could not communicate with them. One way to identify animalpersonality traits is to observe the consistency of the individual's behavior over time and circumstances.[44][45] For group-living species, there are two opposing hypotheses regarding the assortment of personalities within a group: the social niche specialization hypothesis, and the conformity hypothesis. To test these two hypotheses, McCune et al. (2018) performed an experiment on the boldness of two species in Corvidae: theMexican Jay andCalifornia Scrub-Jay. Their results confirmed the conformity hypothesis, supported by the significant differences in the group effects.[46]
The individual personality is both determined by genetics and shaped bysocial contexts.[47] Miller et al. (2016) examined the role of the developmental and social environment in personality formation in common ravens and carrion crows, which are highly social corvids. The researchers highlighted the correlation between social contexts and an individual's consistent behavior over time (personality), by showing that conspecific presence promoted the behavioral similarities between individuals. Therefore, the researchers demonstrated that social contexts had a significant impact on the development of the raven's and crow's personalities.[48]
Thesocial complexity hypothesis suggests that living in a social group enhances the cognitive abilities of animals. Corvid ingenuity is represented through their feeding skills,memorization abilities, use of tools, and group behaviour. Living in large social groups has long been connected with high cognitive ability. To live in a large group, a member must be able to recognize individuals, and track the social position and foraging of other members over time. Members must also be able to distinguish between sex, age, reproductive status, and dominance, and to update this information constantly. It might be that social complexity corresponds to their high cognition, as well as contributing to the spread of information between members of the group.[49]
A study published in 2008 suggested that theEurasian magpie is the only non-mammal species known to be able to recognize itself in amirror test,[50] but later research could not replicate this finding.[51] Studies using very similar setups could not find such behaviour in other corvids (e.g., Carrion crows[52][53]). Magpies have been observed taking part in elaborate grieving rituals, which have been likened to humanfunerals, including laying grass wreaths.[54] Marc Bekoff, at theUniversity of Colorado, argues that it shows that they are capable of feeling complex emotions, includinggrief.[54] Furthermore,carrion crows show a neuronal response that correlates with theirperception of a stimulus, which some scientists have argued to be an empirical marker of (avian/corvid)sensory consciousness—the conscious perception of sensory input—in the crows which do not have acerebral cortex.[55][56] A related study shows that thebirds' pallium's neuroarchitecture is reminiscent of the mammalian cortex.[57][58]
ANew Caledonian crow uses a tool to retrieve the correct tool to obtain food.
There are also specific examples of corvid cleverness. Onecarrion crow was documented cracking nuts by placing them on a crosswalk, letting the passing cars crack the shell, waiting for the light to turn red, and then safely retrieving the contents.[59] A group of crows in England took turns lifting garbage bin lids while their companions collected food.[citation needed]
Members of the corvid family have been known to watch other birds, remember where they hide their food, then return once the owner leaves.[60][61] Corvids also move their food around between hiding places to avoid thievery—but only if they have previously been thieves themselves (that is, they remember previous relevant social contexts, use their own experience of having been a thief to predict the behavior of a pilferer, and can determine the safest course to protect their caches from being pilfered). Studies to assess similar cognitive abilities in apes have been inconclusive.[62]
The ability to hide food requires highly accuratespatial memories. Corvids have been recorded to recall their food's hiding places up to nine months later. It is suggested that vertical landmarks (like trees) are used to remember locations. There has also been evidence thatCalifornia scrub jays, which store perishable foods, not only remember where they stored their food, but for how long. This has been compared toepisodic memory, previously thought unique to humans.[3]
New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) are notable for their highly developed tool fabrication. They make angling tools of twigs and leaves trimmed into hooks, and then subsequently use the hooks to pull insect larvae from tree holes. Tools are engineered according to task, and apparently, also to learned preferences. Recent studies revealed abilities to solve complicated problems, which suggested high levels of innovation of a complex nature.[63][64] Other corvids that have been observed using tools include: theAmerican crow,blue jay, andgreen jay. Researchers have discovered thatNew Caledonian crows do not just use single objects as tools—they can also construct novel compound tools through assemblage of otherwise non-functional elements.[65][66] Diversity in tool design among corvids suggests cultural variation. Again, great apes are the only other animals known to use tools in such a fashion.[3]
Clark's nutcrackers andjackdaws were compared in a 2002 study based on geometric rule learning. The corvids, along with adomestic pigeon, had to locate a target between two landmarks, while distances and landmarks were altered. The nutcrackers were more accurate in their searches than the jackdaws and pigeons.[67]
Implications and specific comparisons with other animals
Proportions of correct responses as a function of species (R: ravens; C: chimpanzees; O: orang-utans)[68]
Thescarecrow is an archetypal scare tactic in the agricultural business. However, due to corvids' quick wit, scarecrows are soon ignored, and used as perches. Despite farmers' efforts to rid themselves of corvid pests, their attempts have only expanded corvid territories, and strengthened their numbers.[19]
Contrary to earlierteleological classifications, in which they were seen as "highest" songbirds due to their intelligence, currentsystematics might place corvids—based on their total number of physical characteristics, instead of just their brains (which are the most developed of birds)—in the lower middle of the passerine evolutionary tree, dependent on which subgroup is chosen as the most derived.[11] As per one observer:
During the 19th century, there arose the belief that these were the "most advanced" birds, based upon the belief that Darwinian evolution brings "progress." In such a classification, the "most intelligent" of birds were listed last, reflecting their position "atop the pyramid." Modern biologists reject the concept of hierarchical "progress" in evolution [...].[2]
A study found that four-month-old ravens can have physical and social cognitive skills similar to that of adult great apes, and concluded that the "dynamic of the different influences that, duringontogeny, contributes to adult cognition" is required for the study of cognition.[69][68]
Corvids are reservoirs (carriers) for theWest Nile virus in the United States. They are infected by mosquitoes (thevectors), primarily of theCulex species.Crows andravens are quickly killed by this disease, so their deaths are an early-warning system when West Nile virus arrives in an area (as are horses and other bird-species deaths). One of the first signs that West Nile virus first arrived in the US in 1999 was the death of crows inNew York.[70]
Several different corvids, particularlyravens, have occasionally served aspets, although they are not able to speak as readily asparrots, and are not suited to a caged environment.
Humans have been able to coexist with many members of the Corvidae family throughout history, most notablycrows andravens (see: "Role in myth and culture" section below). These positive interactions have extended into modern times.
Folklore often represents corvids as clever, and even mystical, animals. SomeNative Americans, such as theHaida, believed that a raven created the earth, and despite being a trickster spirit, ravens were popular ontotems, credited with creating man, and considered responsible for placing the Sun in the sky.[71]
Due to theircarrion diet, theCeltic peoples strongly associated corvids with war, death, and the battlefield; their great intelligence meant that they were often considered messengers, or manifestations of the gods, such asBendigeidfran (Welsh for "Blessed Crow") or the IrishMorrigan (Middle Irish for "Great Queen"),[72] both who wereunderworld deities that may be related to the later ArthurianFisher King. The WelshDream of Rhonabwy illustrates well the association of ravens with war. In many parts of Britain, gatherings of crows, or more often magpies, are counted using thedivination rhyme: "one for sorrow, two for joy, three for a girl, four for a boy, five for silver, six for gold, seven for a secret never to be told." Another rhyme is: "one for sorrow, two for mirth, three for a funeral, four for a birth, five for heaven, six for hell, and seven for the Devil, his own sel."Cornish superstition holds that when a lone magpie is encountered, it must be loudly greeted with respect.
VariousGermanic peoples highly revered the raven, and the raven was often depicted as a motif on shields or other war gear inAnglo-Saxon art, such as theSutton Hoo burial, andVendel period art. The major deity,Odin, was so commonly associated with ravens throughout history that he gained thekenning "Raven God,"[b] and theraven banner was the flag of variousViking AgeScandinavian chieftains. Odin was also attended byHugin and Munin, tworavens who flew all over the world, and whispered information they acquired into his ears.[73] TheValravn sometimes appeared in modern Scandinavian folklore. On a shield and purse lid excavated among theSutton Hoo treasures, imagery of stylised corvids with scrolled beaks are meticulously detailed in the decorative enamel work. The corvid symbolism reflected their commontotemic status to the Anglo-Saxons, whose pre-Christianindigenous beliefs were of the same origin as that of the aforementioned Vikings.
The sixth century BCEGreek scribeAesop featured corvids as intelligent antagonists in many fables. Later, in western literature, popularized by American poetEdgar Allan Poe's work "The Raven", thecommon raven becomes a symbol of the main character's descent into madness.
Unlike many other birdfamilies, corvidfitness and reproduction, especially with many crows, has increased due to human development. The survival and reproductive success of certain crows and ravens is assisted by their close relationship with humans.[20]
Human development provides additional resources by clearing land, creating shrublands rich in berries and insects. When the cleared land naturally replenishes, jays and crows use the young dense trees for nesting sites. Ravens typically use larger trees in denser forest.[20]
Most corvids are not threatened, and many species are even increasing in population due to human activity. However, a few species are in danger. For example, the destruction of the Southeast Asian rainforest is endangeringmixed-species feeding flocks with members from the family Corvidae.[74] Also, since itssemiarid scrublandhabitat is an endangeredecosystem, theFlorida scrub jay has a small and declining population.[75][76] A number of island species, which are more vulnerable tointroduced species and habitat loss, have been driven to extinction, such as theNew Zealand raven, or are threatened, like theMariana crow.
TheAmerican crow population of the United States has grown over the years. It is possible that the American crow, due to humans increasing suitable habitat, will causeNorthwestern crows andfish crows to decline.[77]
^Bock, Walter J. (1994).History and Nomenclature of Avian Family-Group Names. Bulletin of the American Museum of Natural History. Vol. 222. New York: American Museum of Natural History. pp. 118, 222.hdl:2246/830.
^abcdeMarzluff, John M.; Neatherlin, Eric (2006). "Corvid response to human settlements and campgrounds: Causes, consequences, and challenges for conservation".Biological Conservation.130 (2):301–314.Bibcode:2006BCons.130..301M.doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2005.12.026.
^Gill, F.B. (2003)Ornithology (2nd edition). W.H. Freeman and Company, New York.ISBN0-7167-2415-4
^Patterson, I. J.; Dunnet, G. M.; Fordham, R. A. (1971). "Ecological studies of the RookCorvus frugilegus L. in northeast Scotland. Dispersion".J. Appl. Ecol.8 (3):815–833.Bibcode:1971JApEc...8..815P.doi:10.2307/2402685.JSTOR2402685.
^Li, Shou-Hsien; Brown, Jerram L. (2000). "High frequency of extrapair fertilization in a plural breeding bird, the Mexican jay, revealed by DNA microsatellites".Animal Behaviour.60 (6):867–877.doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1554.PMID11124886.S2CID34041075.
^Krushinskii, L. V.; Zorina, Z. A.; Dashevskiy, B. A. (1979). "Ability of birds of the Corvidae family to operate by the empirical dimensions of figures".Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova.29 (3):590–597.PMID112801.
^Soler, M.; Colmero, J. M.; Pérez-Contreras, T.; Peralta-Sánchez, J. M. (2020). "Replication of the mirror mark test experiment in the magpie (Pica pica) does not provide evidence of self-recognition".Journal of Comparative Psychology.134 (4):363–371.doi:10.1037/com0000223.PMID32406720.S2CID218636079.
^Lee, T. M.; Soh, M. C. K.; Sodhi, N.; Koh, L. P.; Lim, S. L. H. (2005). "Effects of habitat disturbance on mixed species bird flocks in a tropical sub-montane rainforest".Biological Conservation.122 (2):193–204.Bibcode:2005BCons.122..193L.doi:10.1016/j.biocon.2004.07.005.