Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
Thehttps:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

NIH NLM Logo
Log inShow account info
Access keysNCBI HomepageMyNCBI HomepageMain ContentMain Navigation
pubmed logo
Advanced Clipboard
User Guide

Full text links

Nature Publishing Group full text link Nature Publishing Group Free PMC article
Full text links

Actions

Share

doi: 10.1038/srep40700.

Neurotypical Peers are Less Willing to Interact with Those with Autism based on Thin Slice Judgments

Affiliations

Neurotypical Peers are Less Willing to Interact with Those with Autism based on Thin Slice Judgments

Noah J Sasson et al. Sci Rep..

Abstract

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), including those who otherwise require less support, face severe difficulties in everyday social interactions. Research in this area has primarily focused on identifying the cognitive and neurological differences that contribute to these social impairments, but social interaction by definition involves more than one person and social difficulties may arise not just from people with ASD themselves, but also from the perceptions, judgments, and social decisions made by those around them. Here, across three studies, we find that first impressions of individuals with ASD made from thin slices of real-world social behavior by typically-developing observers are not only far less favorable across a range of trait judgments compared to controls, but also are associated with reduced intentions to pursue social interaction. These patterns are remarkably robust, occur within seconds, do not change with increased exposure, and persist across both child and adult age groups. However, these biases disappear when impressions are based on conversational content lacking audio-visual cues, suggesting that style, not substance, drives negative impressions of ASD. Collectively, these findings advocate for a broader perspective of social difficulties in ASD that considers both the individual's impairments and the biases of potential social partners.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Group effects on each modality. Groups only did not significantly differ on the transcript of Speech Content.
Figure 2
Figure 2. Group effects on each item. ASD group is rated significantly less favorable on every item except Live Near, Trustworthy, and Smart.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Individuals with ASD (red) are immediately judged more negatively than TD controls (blue) in each of the three rating categories (allps < 0.001).
Figure 4
Figure 4. Ratings are very stable over repeated exposure to the same person. Each line represents the mean rating provided for one stimulus participant across image exposure number (blue = TD; red = ASD).
Furthermore, note that ratings of stimulus participants are highly consistent across these three judgments tasks (i.e., individuals who scored high on awkwardness scored low on approachability and friendship, and vice versa). For example, the one ASD participant with very typical ratings for awkwardness is the same participant that is rated high on the approachability and friendship ratings.
Figure 5
Figure 5. Ratings for ASD stimulus participants (red) are all significantly less favorable than for TD controls (blue). Allps < 0.001.
See this image and copyright information in PMC

Similar articles

See all similar articles

Cited by

See all "Cited by" articles

References

    1. Tobin M. C., Drager K. D. R. & Richardson L. F. A systematic review of social participation for adults with autism spectrum disorders: support, social functioning, and quality of life. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 8, 214–229 (2014).
    1. Billstedt E., Gillberg C. & Gillberg C. Autism after adolescence: population-based 13- to-22 year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. 35, 351–360 (2005). - PubMed
    1. Shattuck P. T., Narendorf S. C., Cooper B., Sterzing P. R., Wagner M. & Taylor J. L. Postsecondary education and employment among youth with an autism spectrum disorder. Pediatr. 129, 1042–1049 (2012). - PMC - PubMed
    1. Bauminger N. & Kasari C. Loneliness and friendship in high-functioning children with autism. Child Dev. 71, 447 (2000). - PubMed
    1. Howlin P., Moss P., Savage S. & Rutter M. Social outcomes in mid-to later adulthood among individuals diagnosed with autism and average nonverbal IQ as children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 52, 572–581 (2013). - PubMed

MeSH terms

Related information

Grants and funding

LinkOut - more resources

Full text links
Nature Publishing Group full text link Nature Publishing Group Free PMC article
Cite
Send To

NCBI Literature Resources

MeSHPMCBookshelfDisclaimer

The PubMed wordmark and PubMed logo are registered trademarks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS). Unauthorized use of these marks is strictly prohibited.


[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp