Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


PhilPapersPhilPeoplePhilArchivePhilEventsPhilJobs
Order:

1 filter applied
  1.  68
    The early origins of goal attribution in infancy.Ildikó Király,Bianca Jovanovic,Wolfgang Prinz,Gisa Aschersleben &György Gergely -2003 -Consciousness and Cognition 12 (4):752-769.
    We contrast two positions concerning the initial domain of actions that infants interpret as goal-directed. The 'narrow scope' view holds that goal-attribution in 6- and 9-month-olds is restricted to highly familiar actions (such as grasping) (). The cue-based approach of the infant's 'teleological stance' (), however, predicts that if the cues of equifinal variation of action and a salient action effect are present, young infants can attribute goals to a 'wide scope' of entities including unfamiliar human actions and actions of (...) novel objects lacking human features. It is argued that previous failures to show goal-attribution to unfamiliar actions were due to the absence of these cues. We report a modified replication of showing that when a salient action-effect is presented, even young infants can attribute a goal to an unfamiliar manual action. This study together with other recent experiments reviewed support the 'wide scope' approach indicating that if the cues of goal-directedness are present even 6-month-olds attribute goals to unfamiliar actions. (shrink)
    Direct download(4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  2.  34
    Do infants bind mental states to agents?Dora Kampis,Eszter Somogyi,Shoji Itakura &Ildikó Király -2013 -Cognition 129 (2):232-240.
  3.  23
    Attention in naïve psychology.Fruzsina Elekes &Ildikó Király -2021 -Cognition 206 (C):104480.
    No categories
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  4.  36
    Young Children Selectively Imitate Models Conforming to Social Norms.Katalin Oláh &Ildikó Király -2019 -Frontiers in Psychology 10.
  5.  36
    3-Year-Old Children Selectively Generalize Object Functions Following a Demonstration from a Linguistic In-group Member: Evidence from the Phenomenon of Scale Error.Katalin Oláh,Fruzsina Elekes,Réka Pető,Krisztina Peres &Ildikó Király -2016 -Frontiers in Psychology 7:191432.
    The present study investigated 3-year-old children’s learning processes about object functions. We built on children’s tendency to commit scale errors with tools to explore whether they would selectively endorse object functions from a linguistic in-group over an out-group model. Participants ( n = 37) were presented with different object sets, and a model speaking either in their native or a foreign language demonstrated how to use the presented tools. In the test phase, children received the object sets with two modifications: (...) the original tool was replaced by one that was too big to achieve the goal but was otherwise identical, and another tool was added to the set that looked different but was appropriately scaled for goal attainment. Children in the Native language condition were significantly more likely to commit scale errors – that is, choose the over-sized tool – than children in the Foreign language condition (48 vs. 30%). We propose that these results provide insight into the characteristics of human-specific learning processes by showing that children are more likely to generalize object functions to a category of artifacts following a demonstration from an in-group member. (shrink)
    Direct download(6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  6.  19
    If you presume relevance, you don't need a bifocal lens.Nazlı Altınok,Denis Tatone,Ildikó Király,Christophe Heintz &György Gergely -2022 -Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45:e250.
    We argue for a relevance-guided learning mechanism to account for both innovative reproduction and faithful imitation by focusing on the role of communication in knowledge transmission. Unlike bifocal stance theory, this mechanism does not require a strict divide between instrumental and ritual-like actions, and the goals they respectively fulfill (material vs. social/affiliative), to account for flexibility in action interpretation and reproduction.
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  21
    Visual Fixation Patterns During Viewing of Half-Face Stimuli in Adults: An Eye-Tracking Study.Ágoston Galambos,Borbála Turcsán,Katalin Oláh,Fruzsina Elekes,Anna Gergely,Ildikó Király &József Topál -2018 -Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  29
    Encoding third-person epistemic states contributes to episodic reconstruction of memories.Dora Kampis,András Keszei &Ildikó Király -2018 -Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41:e18.
    We propose an extension to Mahr & Csibra's (M&C's) theory. For successful episodic memory formation, potentially relevant aspects of a situation need to be identified and encodedonlineand retained for prospective interactions.To be maximally convincing, the communicator not only has to encode not justanycontextual detail, but also has to track informationin relation tosocial partners.
    No categories
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  48
    Shifting ''goals'': Clarifying some misconceptions about the teleological stance in young infants.Ildikó Király &György Gergely -2003 -Consciousness and Cognition 12 (4):773-776.
  10.  23
    Evidence of relational retrieval, even in the absence of the relational eye movement effect.Márton Nagy &Ildikó Király -2018 -Consciousness and Cognition 66:40-53.
  11.  12
    Can group representations based on relational cues warrant the rich inferences typically drawn from group membership?Katalin Oláh &Ildikó Király -2022 -Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45.
    Pietraszewski's model – though promising in many respects – needs to be extended so that it can explain the multitude of rich inferences that people draw from group membership. In this commentary, we highlight some facets of group thinking, especially from the field of developmental psychology, that cannot be unambiguously accounted for by a model that is built solely on relational cues.
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  50
    Some cognitive tools for word learning: The role of working memory and goal preference.Mihály Racsmány,Ágnes Lukács,Csaba Pléh &Ildikó Király -2001 -Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (6):1115-1117.
    We propose that Bloom's focus on cognitive factors involved in word learning still lacks a broader perspective. We emphasize the crucial relevance of working memory in learning elements of language. Specifically, we demonstrate through our data that in impaired populations knowledge of some linguistic elements can be dissociated according to the subcomponent of working memory (visual or verbal) involved in a task. Further, although Bloom's concentration on theory of mind as a precondition for word learning is certainly correct, theory of (...) mind being a necessary condition does not make it a sufficient one. On the basis of our studies we point out the importance of a theory of mind related goal preference in acquiring spatial language. In general, we claim that more specific cognitive preferences and constraints should be outlined in detail for the preconditions of acquiring linguistic elements. (shrink)
    Direct download(6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
Export
Limit to items.
Filters





Configure languageshere.Sign in to use this feature.

Viewing options


Open Category Editor
Off-campus access
Using PhilPapers from home?

Create an account to enable off-campus access through your institution's proxy server or OpenAthens.


[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp