Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


PhilPapersPhilPeoplePhilArchivePhilEventsPhilJobs
Switch to: References

Add citations

You mustlogin to add citations.
  1. How Redundant Are Redundant Color Adjectives? An Efficiency-Based Analysis of Color Overspecification.Paula Rubio-Fernández -2016 -Frontiers in Psychology 7.
  • Reductionism about understanding why.Insa Lawler -2016 -Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 116 (2):229-236.
    Paulina Sliwa (2015) argues that knowing why p is necessary and sufficient for understanding why p. She tries to rebut recent attacks against the necessity and sufficiency claims, and explains the gradability of understanding why in terms of knowledge. I argue that her attempts do not succeed, but I indicate more promising ways to defend reductionism about understanding why throughout the discussion.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Overspecification of color, pattern, and size: salience, absoluteness, and consistency.Sammie Tarenskeen,Mirjam Broersma &Bart Geurts -2015 -Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  • The Effect of Scene Variation on the Redundant Use of Color in Definite Reference.Ruud Koolen,Martijn Goudbeek &Emiel Krahmer -2013 -Cognitive Science 37 (2):395-411.
    This study investigates to what extent the amount of variation in a visual scene causes speakers to mention the attribute color in their definite target descriptions, focusing on scenes in which this attribute is not needed for identification of the target. The results of our three experiments show that speakers are more likely to redundantly include a color attribute when the scene variation is high as compared with when this variation is low (even if this leads to overspecified descriptions). We (...) argue that these findings are problematic for existing algorithms that aim to automatically generate psychologically realistic target descriptions, such as the Incremental Algorithm, as these algorithms make use of a fixed preference order per domain and do not take visual scene variation into account. (shrink)
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Toward a Computational Psycholinguistics of Reference Production.Kees van Deemter,Albert Gatt,Roger P. G. van Gompel &Emiel Krahmer -2012 -Topics in Cognitive Science 4 (2):166-183.
    This article introduces the topic ‘‘Production of Referring Expressions: Bridging the Gap between Computational and Empirical Approaches to Reference’’ of the journal Topics in Cognitive Science. We argue that computational and psycholinguistic approaches to reference production can benefit from closer interaction, and that this is likely to result in the construction of algorithms that differ markedly from the ones currently known in the computational literature. We focus particularly on determinism, the feature of existing algorithms that is perhaps most clearly at (...) odds with psycholinguistic results, discussing how future algorithms might include non-determinism, and how new psycholinguistic experiments could inform the development of such algorithms. (shrink)
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Talker-Specific Generalization of Pragmatic Inferences based on Under- and Over-Informative Prenominal Adjective Use.Amanda Pogue,Chigusa Kurumada &Michael K. Tanenhaus -2015 -Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download(5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Stored object knowledge and the production of referring expressions: the case of color typicality.Hans Westerbeek,Ruud Koolen &Alfons Maes -2015 -Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Direct download(5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Reference Production as Search: The Impact of Domain Size on the Production of Distinguishing Descriptions.Gatt Albert,Krahmer Emiel,van Deemter Kees &P. G. van Gompel Roger -2017 -Cognitive Science 41 (S6):1459-1492.
    When producing a description of a target referent in a visual context, speakers need to choose a set of properties that distinguish it from its distractors. Computational models of language production/generation usually model this as a search process and predict that the time taken will increase both with the number of distractors in a scene and with the number of properties required to distinguish the target. These predictions are reminiscent of classic findings in visual search; however, unlike models of reference (...) production, visual search models also predict that search can become very efficient under certain conditions, something that reference production models do not consider. This paper investigates the predictions of these models empirically. In two experiments, we show that the time taken to plan a referring expression—as reflected by speech onset latencies—is influenced by distractor set size and by the number of properties required, but this crucially depends on the discriminability of the properties under consideration. We discuss the implications for current models of reference production and recent work on the role of salience in visual search. (shrink)
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Generation of Referring Expressions: Assessing the Incremental Algorithm.Kees van Deemter,Albert Gatt,Ielka van der Sluis &Richard Power -2012 -Cognitive Science 36 (5):799-836.
    A substantial amount of recent work in natural language generation has focused on the generation of ‘‘one-shot’’ referring expressions whose only aim is to identify a target referent. Dale and Reiter's Incremental Algorithm (IA) is often thought to be the best algorithm for maximizing the similarity to referring expressions produced by people. We test this hypothesis by eliciting referring expressions from human subjects and computing the similarity between the expressions elicited and the ones generated by algorithms. It turns out that (...) the success of the IA depends substantially on the ‘‘preference order’’ (PO) employed by the IA, particularly in complex domains. While some POs cause the IA to produce referring expressions that are very similar to expressions produced by human subjects, others cause the IA to perform worse than its main competitors; moreover, it turns out to be difficult to predict the success of a PO on the basis of existing psycholinguistic findings or frequencies in corpora. We also examine the computational complexity of the algorithms in question and argue that there are no compelling reasons for preferring the IA over some of its main competitors on these grounds. We conclude that future research on the generation of referring expressions should explore alternatives to the IA, focusing on algorithms, inspired by the Greedy Algorithm, which do not work with a fixed PO. (shrink)
    Direct download(5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Cognitive Modeling of Individual Variation in Reference Production and Comprehension.Petra Hendriks -2016 -Frontiers in Psychology 7.
  • Visual Complexity and Its Effects on Referring Expression Generation.Micha Elsner,Alasdair Clarke &Hannah Rohde -2018 -Cognitive Science 42 (S4):940-973.
    Speakers’ perception of a visual scene influences the language they use to describe it—which objects they choose to mention and how they characterize the relationships between them. We show that visual complexity can either delay or facilitate description generation, depending on how much disambiguating information is required and how useful the scene's complexity can be in providing, for example, helpful landmarks. To do so, we measure speech onset times, eye gaze, and utterance content in a reference production experiment in which (...) the target object is either unique or non-unique in a visual scene of varying size and complexity. Speakers delay speech onset if the target object is non-unique and requires disambiguation, and we argue that this reflects the cost of deciding on a high-level strategy for describing it. The eye-tracking data demonstrate that these delays increase when speakers are able to conduct an extensive early visual search, implying that when speakers scan too little of the scene early on, they may decide to begin speaking before becoming aware that their description is underspecified. Speakers’ content choices reflect the visual makeup of the scene—the number of distractors present and the availability of useful landmarks. Our results highlight the complex role of visual perception in reference production, showing that speakers can make good use of complexity in ways that reflect their visual processing of the scene. (shrink)
    No categories
    Direct download(3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Giving Good Directions: Order of Mention Reflects Visual Salience.Alasdair D. F. Clarke,Micha Elsner &Hannah Rohde -2015 -Frontiers in Psychology 6.
  • Collaboratively built semi-structured content and Artificial Intelligence: The story so far.Eduard Hovy,Roberto Navigli &Simone Paolo Ponzetto -2013 -Artificial Intelligence 194 (C):2-27.
  • What's new? A comprehension bias in favor of informativity.Hannah Rohde,Richard Futrell &Christopher G. Lucas -2021 -Cognition 209 (C):104491.
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Color in Reference Production: The Role of Color Similarity and Color Codability.Jette Viethen,Thomas van Vessem,Martijn Goudbeek &Emiel Krahmer -2017 -Cognitive Science 41 (S6):1493-1514.
    It has often been observed that color is a highly preferred attribute for use in distinguishing descriptions, that is, referring expressions produced with the purpose of identifying an object within a visual scene. However, most of these observations were based on visual displays containing only colors that were maximally different in hue and for which the language of experimentation possessed basic color terms. The experiments described in this paper investigate whether speakers’ preference for color is reduced if the color of (...) the target referent is similar to that of the distractors. Because colors that look similar are often also harder to distinguish linguistically, we also examine the impact of the codability of color values. As a third factor, we investigate the salience of available alternative attributes and its impact on the use of color. The results of our experiments show that, while speakers are indeed less likely to use color when the colors in a display are similar, this effect is mostly due to the difficulty in naming similar colors. Color use for color with a basic color term is affected only when the colors of target and distractors are very similar (yet still distinguishable). The salience of our alternative attribute size, manipulated by varying the difference in size between target and distractors, had no impact on the use of color. (shrink)
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Talking about Relations: Factors Influencing the Production of Relational Descriptions.Adriana Baltaretu,Emiel J. Krahmer,Carel van Wijk &Alfons Maes -2016 -Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Direct download(5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Assessing the Incremental Algorithm: A Response to Krahmer et al.Kees van Deemter,Albert Gatt,Ielka van der Sluis &Richard Power -2012 -Cognitive Science 36 (5):842-845.
    This response discusses the experiment reported in Krahmer et al.’s Letter to the Editor of Cognitive Science. We observe that their results do not tell us whether the Incremental Algorithm is better or worse than its competitors, and we speculate about implications for reference in complex domains, and for learning from ‘‘normal” (i.e., non-semantically-balanced) corpora.
    Direct download(3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Logical separability of labeled data examples under ontologies.Jean Christoph Jung,Carsten Lutz,Hadrien Pulcini &Frank Wolter -2022 -Artificial Intelligence 313 (C):103785.
    Direct download(2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The complexity of definability by open first-order formulas.Carlos Areces,Miguel Campercholi,Daniel Penazzi &Pablo Ventura -2020 -Logic Journal of the IGPL 28 (6):1093-1105.
    In this article, we formally define and investigate the computational complexity of the definability problem for open first-order formulas with equality. Given a logic $\boldsymbol{\mathcal{L}}$, the $\boldsymbol{\mathcal{L}}$-definability problem for finite structures takes as an input a finite structure $\boldsymbol{A}$ and a target relation $T$ over the domain of $\boldsymbol{A}$ and determines whether there is a formula of $\boldsymbol{\mathcal{L}}$ whose interpretation in $\boldsymbol{A}$ coincides with $T$. We show that the complexity of this problem for open first-order formulas is coNP-complete. We also (...) investigate the parametric complexity of the problem and prove that if the size and the arity of the target relation $T$ are taken as parameters, then open definability is $\textrm{coW}[1]$-complete for every vocabulary $\tau $ with at least one, at least binary, relation. (shrink)
    Direct download(3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  

  • [8]ページ先頭

    ©2009-2025 Movatter.jp