Pain motivatesorganisms to withdraw from damaging situations, to protect a damaged body part while it heals, and to avoid similar experiences in the future.[2]Congenital insensitivity to pain may result in reducedlife expectancy.[3] Most pain resolves once thenoxious stimulus is removed and the body has healed, but it may persist despite removal of the stimulus and apparent healing of the body. Sometimes pain arises in the absence of any detectable stimulus, damage or disease.[4]
Pain is the most common reason for physician consultation in most developed countries.[5][6] It is a major symptom in many medical conditions, and can interfere with a person'squality of life and general functioning.[7] People in pain experience impaired concentration,working memory,mental flexibility, problem solving and information processing speed, and are more likely to experience irritability, depression, and anxiety.
Simple pain medications are useful in 20% to 70% of cases.[8] Psychological factors such associal support,cognitive behavioral therapy, excitement, or distraction can affect pain's intensity or unpleasantness.[9][10]
First attested in English in 1297, the wordpeyn comes from theOld Frenchpeine, in turn fromLatinpoena meaning "punishment, penalty"[11][12] (also meaning "torment, hardship, suffering" in Late Latin) and that fromGreek ποινή (poine), generally meaning "price paid, penalty, punishment".[13][14]
In many cases, pain fits into one or a combination of three categories:[15]
Nociceptive pain (caused byinflamed or damaged tissue that activates pain sensors callednociceptors).[16] Nociceptive pain is divided into "superficial" and "deep" pain. Deep pains are divided into two parts: "deep physical" and "deep visceral" pain.[17]
Pain is usually transitory, lasting only until the noxious stimulus is removed or the underlying damage or pathology has healed. But some painful conditions, such asrheumatoid arthritis,peripheral neuropathy,cancer, andidiopathic pain, may persist for years. Pain that lasts a long time is called "chronic" or "persistent", and pain that resolves quickly is called "acute". Traditionally, the distinction between acute and chronic pain has relied upon an arbitrary interval of time between onset and resolution; the two most commonly used markers being 3 months and 6 months since the onset of pain,[23] though some theorists and researchers have placed the transition from acute to chronic pain at 12 months.[24]: 93 Others apply "acute" to pain that lasts less than 30 days, "chronic" to pain of at least six months' duration, and "subacute" to pain that lasts from one to six months.[25] A popular alternative definition of "chronic pain", involving no arbitrarily fixed duration, is "pain that extends beyond the expected period of healing".[23] Chronic pain may be classified as "cancer-related" or "benign."[25]
Allodynia is pain experienced in response to an ordinarily painless stimulus.[26] It has no biological function and is classified by characteristics of the stimuli as cold, heat, touch, pressure or a pinprick.[26][27]
Phantom pain is pain felt in a part of the body that has beenamputated or from which the brain no longer receives signals. It is a type of neuropathic pain.[28]
Theprevalence of phantom pain in upper limb amputees is nearly 82%, and in lower limb amputees is 54%.[28] One study found that eight days after amputation, 72% of patients had phantom limb pain, and six months later, 67% reported it.[29][30] Some amputees experience continuous pain that varies in intensity or quality; others experience several bouts of pain per day, or it may reoccur less often. It is often described as shooting, crushing, burning, or cramping. If the pain is continuous for an extended period, parts of the intact body may become sensitized, so touching them evokes pain in the phantom limb. Phantom limb pain may accompanyurination ordefecation.[31]: 61–69
Local anesthetic injections into the nerves or sensitive areas of the stump may relieve pain for days, weeks, or sometimes permanently, despite the drug wearing off in a matter of hours, and small injections ofhypertonic saline into the soft tissue between vertebrae produces local pain that radiates into the phantom limb for ten minutes or so and may be followed by hours, weeks, or even longer of partial or total relief from phantom pain. Vigorous vibration or electrical stimulation of the stump, or current from electrodes surgically implanted onto the spinal cord, all produce relief in some patients.[31]: 61–69
Mirror box therapy produces the illusion of movement and touch in a phantom limb, which in turn may cause a reduction in pain.[32]
Paraplegia, the loss of sensation and voluntary motor control after serious spinal cord damage, may be accompanied bygirdle pain at the level of the spinal cord damage,visceral pain evoked by a filling bladder or bowel, or, in five to ten percent of people with paraplegia, phantom body pain in areas of complete sensory loss. This phantom body pain is initially described as burning or tingling but may evolve into severe crushing or pinching pain or the sensation of fire running down the legs or of a knife twisting in the flesh. Onset may or may not occur immediately years after the disabling injury. Surgical treatment rarely provides lasting relief.[31]: 61–69
Breakthrough pain is transitory pain that comes on suddenly and is not alleviated by the patient's regularpain management. It is common in cancer patients who often have background pain that is generally well-controlled by medications but who also sometimes experience bouts of severe pain that, from time to time, "breaks through" the medication. The characteristics of breakthroughcancer pain vary from person to person and according to the cause. Management of breakthrough pain can entail intensive use ofopioids, includingfentanyl.[33][34]
A patient and doctor discuss congenital insensitivity to pain.
The ability to experience pain is essential for protection from injury and recognition of the presence of injury. Episodicanalgesia may occur under special circumstances, such as in the excitement of sport or war: a soldier on the battlefield may feel no pain for many hours from a traumatic amputation or other severe injury.[35]
Although unpleasantness is an essential part of theIASP definition of pain,[36] it is possible in some patients to induce a state known as pain asymbolia, described as intense pain devoid of unpleasantness, withmorphine injection orpsychosurgery.[37] Such patients report pain but are not bothered by it; they recognize the sensation of pain but suffer little or not at all.[38] Indifference to pain can also rarely be present from birth; these people have normal nerves on medical investigations, and find pain unpleasant, but do not avoid repetition of the pain stimulus.[3]
Insensitivity to pain may also result from abnormalities in thenervous system. This is usually the result ofacquired damage to the nerves, such asspinal cord injury,diabetes mellitus (diabetic neuropathy), orleprosy in countries where that disease is prevalent.[39] These individuals are at risk of tissue damage and infection due to undiscovered injuries. People with diabetes-related nerve damage, for instance, sustain poorly healing foot ulcers as a result of decreased sensation.[40]
A much smaller number of people are insensitive to pain due to an inborn abnormality of the nervous system, known as "congenital insensitivity to pain".[3] Children with this condition incur carelessly-repeated damage to their tongues, eyes, joints, skin, and muscles. Some die before adulthood, and others have a reduced life expectancy.[41] Most people with congenital insensitivity to pain have one of fivehereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathies (which includesfamilial dysautonomia andcongenital insensitivity to pain with anhidrosis).[42] These conditions feature decreased sensitivity to pain together with other neurological abnormalities, particularly of theautonomic nervous system.[3][42] A very rare syndrome with isolated congenital insensitivity to pain has been linked with mutations in theSCN9A gene, which codes for a sodium channel (Nav1.7) necessary in conducting pain nerve stimuli.[43]
Experimental subjects challenged by acute pain and patients in chronic pain experience impairments in attention control,working memory capacity,mental flexibility, problem solving, and information processing speed.[44] Pain is also associated with increased depression, anxiety, fear, and anger.[45]
If I have matters right, the consequences of pain will include direct physical distress, unemployment, financial difficulties, marital disharmony, and difficulties in concentration and attention…
Although pain is considered to be aversive and unpleasant and is therefore usually avoided, ameta-analysis which summarized and evaluated numerous studies from various psychological disciplines, found a reduction innegative affect. Across studies, participants that were subjected to acute physical pain in the laboratory subsequently reported feeling better than those in non-painful control conditions, a finding which was also reflected in physiological parameters.[47] A potential mechanism to explain this effect is provided by theopponent-process theory.
Before the relatively recent discovery ofneurons and their role in pain, various body functions were proposed to account for pain. There were several competing early theories of pain among the ancient Greeks:Hippocrates believed that it was due to an imbalance invital fluids.[48] In the 11th century,Avicenna theorized that there were a number of feeling senses, including touch, pain, and titillation.[49]
In 1644,René Descartes theorized that pain was a disturbance that passed along nerve fibers until the disturbance reached the brain.[48][50] The work of Descartes and Avicenna prefigured the 19th-century development ofspecificity theory. Specificity theory saw pain as "a specific sensation, with its own sensory apparatus independent of touch and other senses".[51] Another theory that came to prominence in the 18th and 19th centuries wasintensive theory, which conceived of pain not as a unique sensory modality, but an emotional state produced by stronger than normal stimuli such as intense light, pressure or temperature.[52] By the mid-1890s,specificity was backed primarily by physiologists and physicians, and psychologists mostly backed theintensive theory. However, after a series of clinical observations byHenry Head and experiments byMax von Frey, the psychologists migrated tospecificity almost en masse. By the century's end, most physiology and psychology textbooks presented painspecificity as fact.[49][51]
Regions of the cerebral cortex associated with pain
Some sensory fibers do not differentiate betweennoxious and non-noxious stimuli, while others (i.e.,nociceptors) respond only to noxious, high-intensity stimuli. At the peripheral end of the nociceptor, noxious stimuli generate currents that, above a given threshold, sendsignals along the nerve fiber to the spinal cord. The "specificity" (whether it responds to thermal, chemical, or mechanical features of its environment) of a nociceptor is determined by whichion channels it expresses at its peripheral end. So far, dozens of types of nociceptor ion channels have been identified, and their exact functions are still being determined.[53]
The pain signal travels from the periphery to the spinal cord alongA-delta andC fibers. Because the A-delta fiber is thicker than the C fiber, and is thinly sheathed in an electrically insulating material (myelin), it carries its signal faster (5–30 m/s) than the unmyelinated C fiber (0.5–2 m/s).[54] Pain evoked by the A-delta fibers is described as sharp and is felt first. This is followed by a duller pain—often described as burning—carried by the C fibers.[55] These A-delta and C fibers enter the spinal cord viaLissauer's tract and connect with spinal cord nerve fibers in thecentral gelatinous substance of the spinal cord. These spinal cord fibers then cross the cord via theanterior white commissure and ascend in thespinothalamic tract. Before reaching the brain, the spinothalamic tract splits into thelateral,neospinothalamic tract and themedial,paleospinothalamic tract. The neospinothalamic tract carries the fast, sharp A-delta signal to the ventral posterolateral nucleus of thethalamus. The paleospinothalamic tract carries the slow, dull C fiber pain signal. Some of the paleospinothalamic fibers peel off in the brain stem—connecting with the reticular formation or midbrain periaqueductal gray—and the remainder terminate in the intralaminar nuclei of the thalamus.[56]
Pain-related activity in the thalamus spreads to theinsular cortex (thought to embody, among other things, the feeling that distinguishes pain from otherhomeostatic emotions such as itch and nausea) andanterior cingulate cortex (thought to embody, among other things, the affective/motivational element, the unpleasantness of pain),[57] and pain that is distinctly located also activates theprimary andsecondary somatosensory cortex.[58]
Spinal cord fibers dedicated to carrying A-delta fiber pain signals and others that carry both A-delta and C fiber pain signals to thethalamus have been identified. Other spinal cord fibers, known aswide dynamic range neurons, respond to A-delta and C fibers and the much larger, more heavily myelinated A-beta fibers that carry touch, pressure, and vibration signals.[54]
Ronald Melzack andPatrick Wall introduced theirgate control theory in the 1965Science article "Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory".[59] The authors proposed that the thin C and A-delta (pain) and large diameter A-beta (touch, pressure, vibration) nerve fibers carry information from the site of injury to two destinations in thedorsal horn of the spinal cord, and that A-beta fiber signals acting on inhibitory cells in the dorsal horn can reduce the intensity of pain signals sent to the brain.[50]
"sensory-discriminative" (sense of the intensity, location, quality, and duration of the pain),
"affective-motivational" (unpleasantness and urge to escape the unpleasantness) and
"cognitive-evaluative" (cognitions such as appraisal, cultural values, distraction, and hypnotic suggestion).
They theorized that pain intensity (the sensory discriminative dimension) and unpleasantness (the affective-motivational dimension) are not simply determined by the magnitude of the painful stimulus, but "higher" cognitive activities can influence perceived intensity and unpleasantness. Cognitive activities may affect both sensory and affective experience, or they may modify primarily the affective-motivational dimension. Thus, excitement in games or war appears to block both the sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational dimensions of pain, while suggestion and placebos may modulate only the affective-motivational dimension and leave the sensory-discriminative dimension relatively undisturbed.[60] (p. 432)
The paper ends with a call to action: "Pain can be treated not only by trying to cut down the sensory input by anesthetic block, surgical intervention and the like, but also by influencing the motivational-affective and cognitive factors as well."[60] (p. 435)
Pain is part of the body's defense system, producing areflexive retraction from the painful stimulus, and tendencies to protect the affected body part while it heals, and avoid that harmful situation in the future.[61][62] It is an important part of animal life, vital to healthy survival. People withcongenital insensitivity to pain have reducedlife expectancy.[3]
InThe Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution, biologistRichard Dawkins addresses the question of why pain should have the quality of being painful. He describes the alternative as a mental raising of a "red flag". To argue why that red flag might be insufficient, Dawkins argues that drives must compete with one another within living beings. The most "fit" creature would be the one whose pains are well balanced. Those pains which mean certain death when ignored will become the most powerfully felt. The relative intensities of pain, then, may resemble the relative importance of that risk to our ancestors.[a] This resemblance will not be perfect, however, because natural selection can be apoor designer. This may have maladaptive results such assupernormal stimuli.[63]
Pain, however, does not only wave a "red flag" within living beings but may also act as a warning sign and a call for help to other living beings. Especially in humans who readily helped each other in case of sickness or injury throughout their evolutionary history, pain might be shaped by natural selection to be a credible and convincing signal of the need for relief, help, and care.[64]
Idiopathic pain (pain that persists after the trauma or pathology has healed, or that arises without any apparent cause) may be an exception to the idea that pain is helpful to survival, although somepsychodynamic psychologists argue that such pain is psychogenic, enlisted as a protective distraction to keep dangerous emotions unconscious.[65]
In pain science, thresholds are measured by gradually increasing the intensity of a stimulus in a procedure calledquantitative sensory testing which involves such stimuli aselectric current, thermal (heat or cold), mechanical (pressure, touch, vibration),ischemic, or chemical stimuli applied to the subject to evoke a response.[66] The "pain perception threshold" is the point at which the subject begins to feel pain, and the "pain threshold intensity" is the stimulus intensity at which the stimulus begins to hurt. The "pain tolerance threshold" is reached when the subject acts to stop the pain.[66]
A person's self-report is the most reliable measure of pain.[67][68][69] Some health care professionals may underestimate pain severity.[70] A definition of pain widely employed in nursing, emphasizing its subjective nature and the importance of believing patient reports, was introduced byMargo McCaffery in 1968: "Pain is whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever he says it does".[71] To assess intensity, the patient may be asked to locate their pain on a scale of 0 to 10, with 0 being no pain at all, and 10 the worst pain they have ever felt. Quality can be established by having the patient complete theMcGill Pain Questionnaire indicating which words best describe their pain.[7]
The visual analogue scale is a common, reproducible tool in the assessment of pain and pain relief.[72] The scale is a continuous line anchored by verbal descriptors, one for each extreme of pain where a higher score indicates greater pain intensity. It is usually 10 cm in length with no intermediate descriptors as to avoid marking of scores around a preferred numeric value. When applied as a pain descriptor, these anchors are often 'no pain' and 'worst imaginable pain". Cut-offs for pain classification have been recommended as no pain (0–4mm), mild pain (5–44mm), moderate pain (45–74mm) and severe pain (75–100mm).[73][check quotation syntax]
The Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI) is a questionnaire designed to assess thepsychosocial state of a person with chronic pain. Combining the MPI characterization of the person with theirIASP five-category pain profile is recommended for deriving the most useful case description.[23]
Non-verbal people cannot use words to tell others that they are experiencing pain. However, they may be able to communicate through other means, such as blinking, pointing, or nodding.[74]
With a non-communicative person, observation becomes critical, and specific behaviors can be monitored as pain indicators. Behaviors such as facial grimacing and guarding (trying to protect part of the body from being bumped or touched) indicate pain, as well as an increase or decrease in vocalizations, changes in routine behavior patterns and mental status changes. Patients experiencing pain may exhibit withdrawnsocial behavior and possibly experience adecreased appetite and decreased nutritional intake. A change in condition that deviates from baseline, such as moaning with movement or when manipulating a body part, and limitedrange of motion are also potential pain indicators. In patients who possess language but are incapable of expressing themselves effectively, such as those with dementia, an increase in confusion or display of aggressive behaviors or agitation may signal that discomfort exists, and further assessment is necessary. Changes in behavior may be noticed by caregivers who are familiar with the person's normal behavior.[74]
Infants do feel pain, but lack the language needed to report it, and so communicate distress by crying. A non-verbal pain assessment should be conducted involving the parents, who will notice changes in the infant which may not be obvious to the health care provider.Pre-term babies are more sensitive to painful stimuli than those carried to full term.[75]
Another approach, when pain is suspected, is to give the person treatment for pain, and then watch to see whether the suspected indicators of pain subside.[74]
The way in which one experiences and responds to pain is related to sociocultural characteristics, such as gender, ethnicity, and age.[76][77] An aging adult may not respond to pain in the same way that a younger person might. Their ability to recognize pain may be blunted by illness or the use ofmedication. Depression may also keep older adult from reporting they are in pain. Decline inself-care may also indicate the older adult is experiencing pain. They may be reluctant to report pain because they do not want to be perceived as weak, or may feel it is impolite or shameful to complain, or they may feel the pain is a form of deserved punishment.[78][79]
Cultural barriers may also affect the likelihood of reporting pain. Patients may feel that certain treatments go against their religious beliefs. They may not report pain because they feel it is a sign that death is near. Many people fear the stigma of addiction, and avoid pain treatment so as not to be prescribed potentially addicting drugs. Many Asians do not want to lose respect in society by admitting they are in pain and need help, believing the pain should be borne in silence, while other cultures feel they should report pain immediately to receive immediate relief.[75]
Gender can also be a perceived factor in reporting pain.Gender differences can be the result of social and cultural expectations, with, in some cultures, women expected to be more emotional and show pain, and men to be more stoic.[75] As a result, female pain may be at a higher risk of being stigmatized, leading to less urgent treatment of women based on social expectations of their ability to accurately report it.[80] This has been postulated to lead to extended emergency room wait times for women and frequent dismissal of their ability to accurately report pain.[81][82]
Pain is a symptom of many medical conditions. Knowing the time of onset, location, intensity, pattern of occurrence (continuous, intermittent, etc.), exacerbating and relieving factors, and quality (burning, sharp, etc.) of the pain will help the examining physician to accurately diagnose the problem. For example, chest pain described as extreme heaviness may indicatemyocardial infarction, while chest pain described as tearing may indicateaortic dissection.[83][84]
Nociceptive pain is caused by stimulation ofsensory nerve fibers that respond to stimuli approaching or exceeding harmful intensity (nociceptors), and may be classified according to the mode of noxious stimulation. The most common categories are "thermal" (e.g. heat or cold), "mechanical" (e.g. crushing, tearing, shearing, etc.) and "chemical" (e.g.iodine in a cut or chemicals released duringinflammation). Some nociceptors respond to more than one of these modalities and are consequently designated polymodal.
Nociceptive pain may also be classed according to the site of origin and divided into "visceral", "deep somatic" and "superficial somatic" pain.Visceral structures (e.g., the heart, liver and intestines) are highly sensitive to stretch,ischemia andinflammation, but relatively insensitive to other stimuli that normally evoke pain in other structures, such as burning and cutting.Visceral pain is diffuse, difficult to locate and oftenreferred to a distant, usually superficial, structure. It may be accompanied by nausea and vomiting and may be described as sickening, deep, squeezing, and dull.[88]Deep somatic pain is initiated by stimulation of nociceptors in ligaments, tendons, bones, blood vessels,fasciae and muscles, and is dull, aching, poorly-localized pain. Examples includesprains and broken bones.Superficial somatic pain is initiated by activation of nociceptors in the skin or other superficial tissue, and is sharp, well-defined and clearly located. Examples of injuries that produce superficial somatic pain include minor wounds and minor (first degree) burns.[24]
Neuropathic pain is caused by damage or disease affecting any part of thenervous system involved in bodily feelings (thesomatosensory system).[89] Neuropathic pain may be divided into peripheral,central, or mixed (peripheral and central) neuropathic pain.Peripheral neuropathic pain is often described as "burning", "tingling", "electrical", "stabbing", or "pins and needles".[90] Bumping the "funny bone" elicits acute peripheral neuropathic pain.
Nociplastic pain is pain characterized by a changednociception (but without evidence of real or threatened tissue damage, or without disease or damage in thesomatosensory system).[10]
Psychogenic pain, also calledpsychalgia orsomatoform pain, is pain caused, increased or prolonged by mental, emotional or behavioral factors.[92] Headaches, back pain and stomach pain are sometimes diagnosed as psychogenic.[92] Those affected are often stigmatized, because both medical professionals and the general public tend to think that pain from a psychological source is not "real". However, specialists consider that it is no less actual or hurtful than pain from any other source.[37]
People withlong-term pain frequently display psychological disturbance, with elevated scores on theMinnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales ofhysteria, depression andhypochondriasis (the "neurotic triad"). Some investigators have argued that it is this neuroticism that causes acute pain to turn chronic, but clinical evidence points in the other direction, to chronic pain causingneuroticism. When long-term pain is relieved by therapeutic intervention, scores on the neurotic triad andanxiety fall, often to normal levels.Self-esteem, often low in chronic pain patients, also shows improvement once pain has resolved.[31]: 31–32
Sugar (sucrose) when taken by mouth reducespain in newborn babies undergoing some medical procedures (alancing of the heel,venipuncture, andintramuscular injections). Sugar does not remove pain fromcircumcision, and it is unknown if sugar reduces pain for other procedures.[116] Sugar did not affect pain-relatedelectrical activity in the brains of newborns one second after the heel lance procedure.[117] Sweet liquid by mouth moderately reduces the rate and duration of crying caused by immunization injection in children between one and twelve months of age.[118]
Suggestion can significantly affect pain intensity. About 35% of people report marked relief after receiving asaline injection they believed to bemorphine. Thisplacebo effect is more pronounced in people who are prone to anxiety, and so anxiety reduction may account for some of the effect, but it does not account for all of it. Placebos are more effective for intense pain than mild pain; and they produce progressively weaker effects with repeated administration.[31]: 26–28 It is possible for many with chronic pain to become so absorbed in an activity or entertainment that the pain is no longer felt, or is greatly diminished.[31]: 22–23
A number of meta-analyses have foundclinical hypnosis to be effective in controlling pain associated with diagnostic and surgical procedures in both adults and children, as well as pain associated with cancer and childbirth.[119] A 2007 review of 13 studies found evidence for the efficacy of hypnosis in the reduction of chronic pain under some conditions, though the number of patients enrolled in the studies was low, raising issues related to the statistical power to detect group differences, and most lacked credible controls for placebo or expectation. The authors concluded that "although the findings provide support for the general applicability of hypnosis in the treatment of chronic pain, considerably more research will be needed to fully determine the effects of hypnosis for different chronic-pain conditions."[120]
An analysis of the 13 highest quality studies of pain treatment withacupuncture, published in January 2009, concluded there was little difference in the effect of real, fake and no acupuncture.[121] However, more recent reviews have found some benefit.[122][123][124]
Additionally, there is tentative evidence for a few herbal medicines.[125]
For chronic (long-term)lower back pain,spinal manipulation produces tiny,clinically insignificant, short-term improvements in pain and function, compared withsham therapy and other interventions.[126] Spinal manipulation produces the same outcome as other treatments, such as general practitioner care, pain-relief drugs, physical therapy, and exercise, for acute (short-term) lower back pain.[126]
There has been some interest in the relationship betweenvitamin D and pain, but the evidence so far fromcontrolled trials for such a relationship, other than inosteomalacia, is inconclusive.[127]
The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) says that due to a lack of evidence from high quality research, it does not endorse the general use of cannabinoids to treat pain.[128]
Pain is the main reason for visiting anemergency department in more than 50% of cases,[129] and is present in 30% of family practice visits.[130] Severalepidemiological studies have reported widely varying prevalence rates for chronic pain, ranging from 12 to 80% of the population.[131] It becomes more common as people approach death. A study of 4,703 patients found that 26% had pain in the last two years of life, increasing to 46% in the last month.[132]
A survey of 6,636 children (0–18 years of age) found that, of the 5,424 respondents, 54% had experienced pain in the preceding three months. A quarter reported having experienced recurrent or continuous pain for three months or more, and a third of these reported frequent and intense pain. The intensity of chronic pain was higher for girls, and girls' reports of chronic pain increased markedly between ages 12 and 14.[133]
Physical pain is a universal experience, and a strong motivator of human and animal behavior. As such, physical pain is used politically in relation to various issues such aspain management policy,drug control, animal rights or animal welfare, torture, andpain compliance. The deliberate infliction of pain and the medical management of pain are both important aspects ofbiopower, a concept that encompasses the "set of mechanisms through which the basic biological features of the human species became the object of a political strategy".[134]
In various contexts, the deliberate infliction of pain in the form ofcorporal punishment is used as retribution for an offence, for the purpose of disciplining or reforming a wrongdoer, or to deter attitudes or behaviour deemed unacceptable. In Western societies, the intentional infliction of severe pain (torture) was principally used to extract confession prior to its abolition in the latter part of the 19th century. Torture as a means to punish thecitizen has been reserved for offences posing a severe threat to the social fabric (for example,treason).[135]
The administration of torture on bodies othered by the cultural narrative, those observed as not 'full members of society'[135]: 101–121[AD1] met a resurgence in the 20th century, possibly due to the heightened warfare.[135]: 101–121 [AD2]
Beliefs about pain play an important role in sporting cultures. Pain may be viewed positively, exemplified by the 'no pain, no gain' attitude, with pain seen as an essential part of training. Sporting culture tends to normalise experiences of pain and injury and celebrate athletes who 'play hurt'.[138]
Pain has psychological, social, and physical dimensions, and is greatly influenced by cultural factors.[139]
René Descartes argued that animals lack consciousness and therefore do not experience pain and suffering in the way that humans do.[140]Bernard Rollin ofColorado State University, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals,[b] wrote that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and that veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were simply taught to ignore animal pain.[142][143] The ability of invertebrate species of animals, such as insects, to feel pain and suffering is unclear.[144][145][146]
Specialists believe that all vertebrates can feel pain, and that certain invertebrates, like the octopus, may also.[144][147][148] The presence of pain in animals is unknown, but can be inferred through physical and behavioral reactions,[149] such as paw withdrawal from various noxious mechanical stimuli in rodents.[150]
Whileplants, as living beings, can perceive and communicate physical stimuli and damage, they do not feel pain simply because of the lack of any pain receptors, nerves, or a brain,[151] and, by extension, a lack of consciousness.[152] Many plants are known to perceive and respond to mechanical stimuli at a cellular level, and some plants such as thevenus flytrap ortouch-me-not, are known for their "obvious sensory abilities".[151] Nevertheless, no member of the plant kingdom does feel pain notwithstanding their abilities to respond to sunlight, gravity, wind, and any external stimuli such as insect bites since they lack any nervous system. The primary reason for this is that, unlike the members of theanimal kingdom whose evolutionary successes and failures are shaped by suffering, the evolution of plants are simply shaped by life and death.[151]
^Diagnostic Methods for Neuropathic Pain: A Review of Diagnostic Accuracy Rapid Response Report: Summary with Critical Appraisal. Canadian Agency for Drugs and Technologies in Health. April 2015.PMID26180859.
^abcTurk DC, Okifuji A (2001). "Pain terms and taxonomies of pain". In Bonica JJ, Loeser JD, Chapman CR, Turk DC (eds.).Bonica's management of pain. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.ISBN978-0781768276.
^Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P (November 1983). "Phantom limb, phantom pain and stump pain in amputees during the first 6 months following limb amputation".Pain.17 (3):243–256.doi:10.1016/0304-3959(83)90097-0.PMID6657285.S2CID10304696.
^Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P (March 1985). "Immediate and long-term phantom limb pain in amputees: incidence, clinical characteristics and relationship to pre-amputation limb pain".Pain.21 (3):267–278.doi:10.1016/0304-3959(85)90090-9.PMID3991231.S2CID24358789.
^abcdefWall PD, Melzack R (1996).The challenge of pain (2nd ed.). New York: Penguin Books.ISBN978-0140256703.
^Hart RP, Wade JB, Martelli MF (April 2003). "Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic pain: the significance of stress".Current Pain and Headache Reports.7 (2):116–126.doi:10.1007/s11916-003-0021-5.PMID12628053.S2CID14104974.
^abLinton. Models of Pain Perception. Elsevier Health, 2005. Print.
^abDallenbach KM (July 1939). "Pain: History and present status".American Journal of Psychology.52 (3):331–347.doi:10.2307/1416740.JSTOR1416740.
^abMelzack R, Katz J (2004). "The Gate Control Theory: Reaching for the Brain". In Craig KD, Hadjistavropoulos T (eds.).Pain: psychological perspectives. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.ISBN978-0415650618.
^abBonica JJ (1990). "History of pain concepts and therapies".The management of pain. Vol. 1 (2 ed.). London: Lea & Febiger. p. 7.ISBN978-0812111224.
^Finger S (2001).Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. US: Oxford University Press. p. 149.ISBN978-0195146943.
^abMarchand S (2010). "Applied pain neurophysiology". In Beaulieu P, Lussier D, Porreca F, Dickenson A (eds.).Pharmacology of pain. Seattle: International Association for the Study of Pain Press. pp. 3–26.ISBN978-0931092787.
^Romanelli P, Esposito V (July 2004). "The functional anatomy of neuropathic pain".Neurosurgery Clinics of North America.15 (3):257–268.doi:10.1016/j.nec.2004.02.010.PMID15246335.
^abMelzack, Ronald;Casey, Kenneth (1968). "Sensory, Motivational, and Central Control Determinants of Pain". In Kenshalo, Dan (ed.).The Skin Senses. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas.
^Lynn B (1984)."Cutaneous nociceptors". In Winlow W, Holden AV (eds.).The neurobiology of pain: Symposium of the Northern Neurobiology Group, held at Leeds on 18 April 1983. Manchester: Manchester University Press. p. 106.ISBN978-0719009969.Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved3 February 2016.
^Bernston GG, Cacioppo JT (2007)."The neuroevolution of motivation". In Gardner WL, Shah JY (eds.).Handbook of Motivation Science. New York: The Guilford Press. p. 191.ISBN978-1593855680.Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved18 November 2020.
^Amico D (2016).Health & physical assessment in nursing. Boston: Pearson. p. 173.ISBN978-0133876406.
^Taylor C (2015).Fundamentals of nursing : the art and science of person-centered nursing care. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer Health. p. 241.ISBN978-1451185614.
^Venes D (2013).Taber's cyclopedic medical dictionary. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis. p. 1716.ISBN978-0803629776.
^Prkachin KM, Solomon PE, Ross J (June 2007). "Underestimation of pain by health-care providers: towards a model of the process of inferring pain in others".The Canadian Journal of Nursing Research.39 (2):88–106.PMID17679587.
^McCaffery M. (1968).Nursing practice theories related to cognition, bodily pain, and man-environment interactions. Los Angeles: UCLA Students Store. More recently, McCaffery defined pain as "whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever the experiencing person says it does."Pasero C, McCaffery M (1999).Pain: clinical manual. St. Louis: Mosby.ISBN978-0815156093.
^Hawker GA, Mian S, Kendzerska T, French M (November 2011). "Measures of adult pain: Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS Pain), Numeric Rating Scale for Pain (NRS Pain), McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ), Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS), Short Form-36 Bodily Pain Scale (SF-36 BPS), and Measure of Intermittent and Constant Osteoarthritis Pain (ICOAP)".Arthritis Care & Research.63 (S11): S240–252.doi:10.1002/acr.20543.PMID22588748.
^abcLewis SM, Bucher L, Heitkemper MM, Harding M (2017).Medical-surgical nursing: Assessment and management of clinical problems (10th ed.). St. Louis, Missouri: Elsevier. p. 126.ISBN978-0323328524.OCLC944472408.
^abcJarvis C (2007).Physical examination & health assessment. St. Louis, Mo: Elsevier Saunders. pp. 180–192.ISBN978-1455728107.
^Encandela JA (March 1993). "Social science and the study of pain since Zborowski: a need for a new agenda".Social Science & Medicine.36 (6):783–791.doi:10.1016/0277-9536(93)90039-7.PMID8480223.
^Encandela JA (1997). "Social Construction of pain and aging: Individual artfulness within interpretive structures".Symbolic Interaction.20 (3):251–273.doi:10.1525/si.1997.20.3.251.
^Lawhorne L, Passerini J (1999).Chronic Pain Management in the Long Term Care Setting: Clinical Practice Guidelines. Baltimore, Maryland: American Medical Directors Association. pp. 1–27.
^Panju AA, Hemmelgarn BR, Guyatt GH, Simel DL (October 1998). "The rational clinical examination. Is this patient having a myocardial infarction?".JAMA.280 (14):1256–1263.doi:10.1001/jama.280.14.1256.PMID9786377.
^Slater EE, DeSanctis RW (May 1976). "The clinical recognition of dissecting aortic aneurysm".The American Journal of Medicine.60 (5):625–633.doi:10.1016/0002-9343(76)90496-4.PMID1020750.
^Urch CE, Suzuki R (26 September 2008). "Pathophysiology of somatic, visceral, and neuropathic cancer pain". In Sykes N, Bennett MI & Yuan C-S (ed.).Clinical pain management: Cancer pain (2d ed.). London: Hodder Arnold. pp. 3–12.ISBN978-0340940075.
^Treede RD, Jensen TS, Campbell JN, Cruccu G, Dostrovsky JO, Griffin JW, Hansson P, Hughes R, Nurmikko T, Serra J (April 2008). "Neuropathic pain: redefinition and a grading system for clinical and research purposes".Neurology.70 (18):1630–1635.doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000282763.29778.59.hdl:11573/97043.PMID18003941.S2CID30172528.
^Thienhaus O, Cole BE (2002). "The classification of pain". In Weiner RS (ed.).Pain management: A practical guide for clinicians. American Academy of Pain Management. p. 29.ISBN978-0849322624.
^Brown AK, Christo PJ, Wu CL (December 2004). "Strategies for postoperative pain management".Best Practice & Research. Clinical Anaesthesiology.18 (4):703–717.doi:10.1016/j.bpa.2004.05.004.PMID15460554.
^Cullen L, Greiner J, Titler MG (June 2001). "Pain management in the culture of critical care".Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America.13 (2):151–166.doi:10.1016/S0899-5885(18)30046-7.PMID11866399.
^Smith GF, Toonen TR (April 2007). "Primary care of the patient with cancer".American Family Physician.75 (8):1207–1214.PMID17477104.
^Jacobson PL, Mann JD (January 2003). "Evolving role of the neurologist in the diagnosis and treatment of chronic noncancer pain".Mayo Clinic Proceedings.78 (1):80–84.doi:10.4065/78.1.80.PMID12528880.
^Selbst SM, Fein JA (2006)."Sedation and analgesia". In Henretig FM, Fleisher GR, Ludwig S (eds.).Textbook of pediatric emergency medicine. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.ISBN978-1605471594.Archived from the original on 11 June 2016. Retrieved3 February 2016.
^Delegates to the International Pain Summit of the International Association for the Study of Pain (2010)."Declaration of Montreal". Archived fromthe original on 13 May 2011. Retrieved7 March 2022.
^Horlocker TT, Cousins MJ, Bridenbaugh PO, Carr DL (2008).Cousins and Bridenbaugh's Neural Blockade in Clinical Anesthesia and Pain Medicine. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.ISBN978-0781773881.
^Chang SC, Hsu CH, Hsu CK, Yang SS, Chang SJ (February 2017). "The efficacy of acupuncture in managing patients with chronic prostatitis/chronic pelvic pain syndrome: A systemic review and meta-analysis".Neurourology and Urodynamics.36 (2):474–481.doi:10.1002/nau.22958.PMID26741647.S2CID46827576.
^Gagnier JJ, Oltean H, van Tulder MW, Berman BM, Bombardier C, Robbins CB (January 2016). "Herbal Medicine for Low Back Pain: A Cochrane Review".Spine.41 (2):116–133.doi:10.1097/BRS.0000000000001310.PMID26630428.
^Cordell WH, Keene KK, Giles BK, Jones JB, Jones JH, Brizendine EJ (May 2002). "The high prevalence of pain in emergency medical care".The American Journal of Emergency Medicine.20 (3):165–169.doi:10.1053/ajem.2002.32643.PMID11992334.
^Perquin CW, Hazebroek-Kampschreur AA, Hunfeld JA, Bohnen AM, van Suijlekom-Smit LW, Passchier J, van der Wouden JC (July 2000). "Pain in children and adolescents: a common experience".Pain.87 (1):51–58.doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00269-4.PMID10863045.S2CID9813003.
^Foucault M (2007).Security, Territory, Population: Lectures at the College de France, 1977–78. Palgrave Macmillan. p. 1.
^Atkinson M, Young K (2001). "Flesh journeys: neo primitives and the contemporary rediscovery of radical body modification".Deviant Behavior.22 (2):117–146.doi:10.1080/016396201750065018.S2CID146525156.
^Loland S, Skirstad B, Waddington I, eds. (2006).Pain and injury in sport: Social and ethical analysis. London and New York: Routledge. pp. 17–20.ISBN978-0415357043.
^Rollin B (1989).The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 117–118. cited inCarbone L (2004).What animals want: expertise and advocacy in laboratory animal welfare policy. US: Oxford University Press. p. 150.
^Lockwood JA (1987). "The Moral Standing of Insects and the Ethics of Extinction".The Florida Entomologist.70 (1):70–89.doi:10.2307/3495093.JSTOR3495093.
^Abbott FV, Franklin KB, Westbrook FR (January 1995). "The formalin test: scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats".Pain.60 (1):91–102.doi:10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V.PMID7715946.S2CID35448280.
^abcPetruzzello, Melissa (2016)."Do Plants Feel Pain?".Encyclopedia Britannica.Archived from the original on 5 September 2023. Retrieved8 January 2023.Given that plants do not have pain receptors, nerves, or a brain, they do not feel pain as we members of the animal kingdom understand it. Uprooting a carrot or trimming a hedge is not a form of botanical torture, and you can bite into that apple without worry.
Casey K (2019).Chasing Pain: The Search for a Neurobiological Mechanism. New York: Oxford University Press.ISBN978-0190880231.
Allison Parshall, "Pain Language: The sound of 'ow' transcends borders",Scientific American, vol. 332, no. 2 (February 2025), pp. 16–18. "Manylanguages have aninterjection word for expressing pain. [Katarzyna Pisanskiet al., writing in theJournal of the Acoustical Society of America, have] found that pain interjections tend to contain thevowel sound 'ah' (written as [a] in theInternational Phonetic Alphabet) and letter combinations that incorporate it, such as 'ow' and 'ai.' These patterns may point back to the origins of human language itself." (p. 16.) "Researchers are continually discovering cases ofsymbolism, or soundiconicity, in which a word's intrinsic nature has some connection to its meaning. These cases run counter to decades oflinguistic theory, which had regarded language as fundamentally arbitrary... [Many wordsonomatopoeically imitate a sound. Also] there's the'bouba-kiki' effect, whereby people from varying cultures are more likely to associate the nonsense word 'bouba' with a rounded shape and 'kiki' with a spiked one.... [S]omehow we all have afeeling about this,' says Aleksandra Ćwiek... [She and her colleagues have] show[n] that people associate thetrilled 'R' sound with roughness and the 'L' sound with smoothness.Mark Dingemanse... in 2013 found [that] the conversational 'Huh?' and similar words in other languages may be universal." (p. 18.)