This articleneeds additional citations forverification. Please helpimprove this article byadding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. Find sources: "Habitual be" – news ·newspapers ·books ·scholar ·JSTOR(January 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this message) |
Habitualbe, also calledinvariantbe, is the use of anuninflectedbe inAfrican-American English (AAE),Caribbean English andHiberno-English to markhabitual or extended actions in place of theStandard English inflected forms ofbe, such asis andare. This is referred to as thehabitual aspect of the verb "to be". In AAE, use ofbe indicates that a subject repeatedly does an action or embodies a trait. InGeneral American English, however, the use of (an inflection of)be means only that an individual has done an action in a particular tense, such as in the statement "She was singing" (the habitual is "She sings").
It is a common misconception that AAE simply replacesis withbe across all tenses, with no added meaning. In fact, AAE usesbe to mark a habitualgrammatical aspect, which is not explicitly distinguished in Standard English. For example,to be singing meansto sing habitually, notto presently be singing. In one experiment, five- and six-year-old children were shown drawings ofElmo eating cookies whileCookie Monster (a character in the popular United States children's television showSesame Street who habitually eats cookies) looked on. Thirty-five of the children were AAE-speaking and 18 were Standard English speaking. Both AAE-speaking and Standard English speaking subjects agreed that Elmois eating cookies, but the AAE-speaking children said that Cookie Monsterbe eating cookies.[1][2][3]
The source of habitualbe in AAE is still disputed. Some linguists suggest it came from the finitebe in the 17th- to 19th-century English of British settlers (perhaps especially those fromSouth West England, but the usage may be the recent "Mummerset" in this context). Other linguists believe that it came fromScots-Irish immigrants, whoseUlster Scots dialects mark habitual verb forms withbe anddo be.
One hypothesis is that habitualbe simply diffused into New World Black English fromHiberno-English (HE) through contact in the Caribbean. Evidence includes the fact that both dialects structure sentences with the habitualbe almost identically:
(1)Even when I do be round there with friends, I do be scared. (HE)
(2)Christmas Day, well, everybody be so choked up over gifts and everything, they don't be too hungry. (AAE)[4]
Criticism of that hypothesis stems from the fact that there is no evidence thatbe has been used as a habitual marker either in the past or today in Caribbeancreoles of English.[4] Instead, Caribbean English uses the preverbaldoes to mark habitualness. They usebe only as filler betweendoes and the sentence's predicate.
The hypothesis states that the geographical differences in use ofbe anddo (be), in Northern and Southern HE respectively, accounts for the difference in use ofbe anddoes (be) in AAE and Caribbean English respectively.[4] In the 17th and 18th centuries, Northern HE speakers immigrated more to North America, and Southern HE speakers immigrated to the Caribbean Islands, both working alongside black people. Although the expansion to include the differences in the dialects of HE accounts for the absence of habitualbe in Caribbean English creoles, the hypothesis has its disadvantages as well. The first problem is the distribution ofdo andbe again. In Southern HE,do is more common withbe than other verbs, but in Caribbean English,does is less common withbe than other verbs.[4] Hibernian English marks habitualness withbe, and Caribbean English rarely marks it, if at all. A second problem is that there is not sufficient evidence to show that Southern HE speakers did not introducedo (be) to the American colonies since there were Southern HE speakers in the colonies who worked closely with Black people.
A further expansion and modification of the diffusion hypotheses account for the periphrasticdo found in Caribbean English creoles. The feature was common in British English and persisted in the nonstandard Southern and Southwestern English dialects that were used by the White colonials in the Caribbean colonies.[4] Irish and Black people both learned English at the same time, and both groups learned a new language and retained the conventions of their native languages. In America, the Irish feature habitualbe may have diffused into AAE and the two assemblages of people were in close contact and communicating with a new tongue. It is possible that British dialects could have had features that served as models for habitualdo (be) in the Caribbean creoles, which, in turn, expanded to AAE in the Americas. Also, Southern and Southwestern British immigrants traveled to the American colonies as well and their dialects would have been used as a model to Black people, leading to a drawback, the introduction, and subsequent loss of habitualdo (be) in America, which was also the problem with the first expansion of the diffusion hypothesis.[4] Another hypothesis for the origins of habitualbe in HE and then into AAE is the most logical and the strongest of the lot.
Thedecreolization theory for the emergence of habitualbe in AAE is the most likely and most supported of the theories of its origin. It involves the decreolization process of Caribbean English creole, with the loss ofdoes (be), which itself is a decreolization of a previous creole habitual marker and its co-occurrence withbe.[4] The process is seen as one code-shifting in the series of English learned by black people in the New World. The existence of a category of habituals in the native Caribbean languages at the basilectal level shows that over time, Black people learned English but kept the conventions of their native languages until code-shifting replaced the old conventions with new ones. Rickford gives evidence:
(18) Habitual aspect with a prepositional phrase or locative:
Stage 1: He (d)a de [dc] in the bed. (basilect)
Stage 2: He does de in the bed. (hab. (d)a -> does)
Stage 3: He does be in the bed. (loc. cop. de -> be)
Stage 4: He 0 be in the bed. (does -> 0; be 'habitual')
That set is just one of three; preceding a predicate containing a prepositional phrase, it shows the shifting from the basilectal, native language convention, level to the English habitualbe level in stage 4, with the co-existence of two or more stages at once.[4] It is hypothesized that the first three stages were present in the speech of plantation slaves, and the hypothesis appears to be supported by the presence of these stages today on theSea Islands of the United States, with only stage four surviving anywhere else in America.[4] The advantages of this hypothesis are that there is no assumption that black people had no native language influence and that the conventions of English were perfectly copied to their emerging English grammars and the fact that the decreolizing of habitualbe also follows the pattern of decreolization in general linguistics and the pattern in cultural anthropology, with formal approximations of English over time and cultural assimilation of language respectively.[4]
Another merit is that this same pattern of decreolizing ofbe is found in other creoles that are relatively close to AAE and affirm the plausibility of this origin for habitualbe. Yet another merit for the hypothesis is that it can both incorporate the strong points of the revised diffusion hypotheses and surmount the weaknesses associated with them. For instance, creoles and dialects have lexicons that derive from the languages that feed them, and AAE and Caribbean English are no different by following the models of language dialects that came in contact with them and used their native language conventions as well as the newly learned conventions to mutate into varieties of the model language.
A possible disadvantage of the hypothesis is that it does not work for areas in which creoles did not develop, such as areas of America with very few black people in the population.[4] Another possible disadvantage is that the sources of slaves for the differing regions of America and the Caribbean could have led to different creole starting points, leading to the different habitual markers in AAE and Caribbean English creoles. Both minor problems are far outweighed by the advantages of the hypothesis, and they affirm that it is the best possible origin of habitualbe.