Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Wikipedia

Marinus van IJzendoorn

Marinus H. "Rien" van IJzendoorn (May 14, 1952) is professor ofhuman development and one of the co-leaders ofGeneration R at theErasmus University Rotterdam. His work has focussed on the social, psychological, and neurobiological determinants ofparenting andchild development, with special emphasis onattachment,emotion regulation,differential susceptibility hypothesis, andchild maltreatment.

Marinus van IJzendoorn
Portrait of Marinus van IJzendoorn
Born (1952-05-14)May 14, 1952 (age 72)
NationalityDutch
Alma materUniversity of Amsterdam,Free University of Berlin
Known forContributions toattachment theory
Scientific career
FieldsChild and family studies
InstitutionsLeiden University,Erasmus University
Thesis (1978)
Websitewww.marinusvanijzendoorn.nlwww.opvoeding-wetenschap.nl

Biography

edit

In 1976, van IJzendoorn graduatedcum laude at theUniversity of Amsterdam. Two years later he obtained hisPhDmagna cum laude at theFree University of Berlin/Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Education. After this he continued his work atLeiden University. In 1981, at age 29, he became full professor.[1] Van IJzendoorn worked as a guest researcher at several places in theUnited States andIsrael. He received a Pionier-grant from theNetherlands Organisation for Scientific Research in 1991,[2] which was used for the start of a new research group. In 1998 he became a member of theRoyal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences.[3] In 1998 and 1999 he was thedean of the Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences. In 2004 Van IJzendoorn received theSpinoza Prize for his work.[4] In 2007 he was awarded for his Distinguished International Contributions to Child Development by theSociety for Research in Child Development,[5] and in 2008 he received anhonorary doctorate at theUniversity of Haifa.[6] In 2011 he received the Aristotle Prize of theEuropean Federation of Psychologists' Associations,[7] and the Bowlby-Ainsworth Founder Award of the Center for Mental Health Promotion and The New York Attachment Consortium.[8] In 2013 he was awarded theDr Hendrik Muller Prize from the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences.[9] He has been a fellow of theAssociation for Psychological Science since 2011.[10] Van IJzendoorn is an ISIThomson Reuters Highly Cited Researcher 2016[11][12] placing him in the top 1% most cited scientists in thepsychology/psychiatry world over the years 2004–2014.[13]

Van IJzendoorn was one of the founders of the Lolle Nauta Foundation (now extinct)[14] that aimed to stimulate the study of education, developmental psychology, and child and family studies on the African continent.

Van IJzendoorn still holds an appointment (until 2022) as professor of human development at the Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, and is a visiting researcher at the University of Cambridge.[15]

Research

edit

Attachment

edit

Van IJzendoorn's main research topic is attachment across the life-span. Attachment has been briefly defined as children's "strong disposition to seek proximity to and contact with a specific figure and to do so in certain situations, notably when they are frightened, tired or ill".[16] Inspired byDarwinianevolutionary theory andHarlow's experimental work withrhesus monkeys,John Bowlby was the first to propose that human genetic selection had favoured attachment behaviours since they increased infant-parent proximity, which in turn enhanced the chances for infant survival. Attachment is considered to be an inborn capacity of every exemplar of the human species. Individual differences in the quality of attachment emerge in the first years of life, and central toattachment theory is the idea that parenting, more specifically parental sensitive responsiveness to the infant's distress signals, determines whether children develop a secure or an insecure attachment relationship with their primary caregiver. Van IJzendoorn introduced in 1995 the quantitative model of the ‘transmission gap’ between parents’ attachments and their infants’ attachment as mediated by parental sensitive responsiveness. This model generated a host of empirical studies trying to bridge the gap (see[17] for a meta-analysis of this work).

Video-feedback intervention

edit

Together with Femmie Juffer andMarian Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn developed and tested In the Video Intervention to promote Positive Parenting (VIPP) program[18] Video feedback provides the opportunity to focus on the infant's videotaped signals and expressions, thereby stimulating the parent's observational skills and empathy for his/her child. It also enablespositive reinforcement of the parent's moments of sensitive behaviour shown on the videotape. In the Video Intervention to promote Positive Parenting-Sensitive Discipline] (VIPP-SD) thematic discussions about limit setting issues are included, based on Patterson's ideas about coercive cycles[19] (see:Morality throughout the Life Span), in order to support parents of ‘terrible two's and three's' to deal with discipline in a consistent and warm manner.

Twenty years of thoroughly testing the VIPP and VIPP-SD programs in randomized trials in various at-risk andclinical groups have demonstrated its efficacy in improving positive parenting skills:[20][21][22][23][24] The combined effect size on parenting sensitivity is substantial.[25] Separate modules have been developed for use with home-based and centre-based child care (VIPP-CC), Turkish-Dutch families (VIPP-TM), and families with a child with Autism Spectrum Disorder (VIPP-AUTI).

Differential susceptibility

edit

The differential susceptibility hypothesis proposes that in positive environments vulnerable children may outperform their peers who turn out to be less susceptible not only to bad environments but also to optimal environments. Moreover, the same characteristics that make individuals vulnerable to adversity also make them disproportionately likely to benefit from contextual support.[26] What characteristics render individuals susceptible to environmental influences? Three broad constructs have been proposed and tested as markers of susceptibility: (a) reactive temperament, (b) biological sensitivity to stress, and (c) genetic make-up. The idea of genetic susceptibility was coined by the Leiden team.[27][28] Importantly, the efficacy of interventions might be underestimated or even go undetected as a main effect when it is hidden in interactions with (genetic, biological or temperamental) characteristics of the recipient of the intervention.[29]

Oxytocin

edit

In humans,oxytocin has been shown to be associated with delivery, mood regulation, sexual functioning and parenting behaviours. Emerging studies are showing an increasing link between oxytocin levels in humans and parent-child relationships. One idea is that increased oxytocin levels facilitatespostnatal parental behaviour and the formation of an emotional bond between parent and infant, in mothers and fathers alike, by acting to reduce anxiety and ameliorate responses to external stresses. Parents who have a less anxious state of mind are able to increase their focus on infant care, improve mood, and facilitate the capacity to readnon-verbal infant cues and stimulate thesocial learning andreward system in response to infant cues. Although there are strong animal models for the role of oxytocin in parental behaviour, one of the key questions in the development of apsychobiology of human care-giving andattachment is the degree to which the considerable variation in parenting can be accounted for through similar biological mechanisms such as the moderating role of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) polymorphisms and experimentally induced oxytocin levels.Van IJzendoorn and colleagues conducted several correlational studies documenting the role of OXTR in parenting, and carried out variousrandomized control trials showing that oxytocin enhances male and female sensitivity to child signals, in natural play settings as well as with a cry test, using behavioural assessments,EEG/ERP, andfMRI. Ameta-analysis of experimental studies withintranasal oxytocin administration was conducted showing that feelings of trust are elevated and that the expected lowering of out-group trust was not confirmed.[30]

Child maltreatment

edit

The first nationwide prevalence study ofchild maltreatment in theNetherlands (NPM-2005) was designed as a replication of the National Incidence Studies conducted in the United States.[31] It was followed by the second National Prevalence Study on Maltreatment (NPM-2010).[32]

At the request of the Dutch Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport and the Dutch Ministry of Justice ("commissie Samsom"), a study on the prevalence ofsexual abuse in youth living in out-of-home care was conducted in the Netherlands in 2008–2010.[33][34][35]

Holocaust

edit

Van IJzendoorn has been personally interested and deeply involved in a series of studies on theHolocaust, including the (long-term) effects on the first, second, and third generation of Holocaust survivors. In one of the first studies that avoided recruitment of participants through convenience groups (e.g., mental health clinics, Holocaust-related organizations, and advertisements), basic population-wide demographic information provided by the Israeli Ministry of the Interior were used to invite participants. It was shown that Holocaust survivors (now grandmothers) had more signs of traumatic stress and, more often, lack of resolution of trauma than comparison subjects, but they were not impaired in their general adaptation. Also, the traumatic effects did not appear to transmit across generations. Thus, Holocaust survivors may have been able to protect their daughters from their war experiences,[36] although they themselves still suffer from the effects of the Holocaust.

In the first population-based retrospective cohort study of theHolocaust, it was tested whether surviving genocidal experiences, like the Holocaust, leads to shorter life-expectancy. However, against all odds, it was found that genocidal survivors were likely to live longer. Differential mortality during the Holocaust and "posttraumatic growth" associated with protective factors in Holocaust survivors or in their environment after World War II were suggested as explanations for these findings.[37]

Selected publications

edit

References

edit
  1. ^"Marinus van IJzendoorn - Universiteit Leiden".
  2. ^Marianne."Prof. dr. M.H. (Rien) van IJzendoorn".
  3. ^"Rien van IJzendoorn". Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. Archived fromthe original on 27 April 2016. Retrieved27 April 2016.
  4. ^"Prof. M.H. (Rien) van IJzendoorn". Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research. Retrieved27 April 2016.
  5. ^"Distinguished International Contributions to Child Development"(PDF). 2007. p. 3. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2 June 2016.
  6. ^"Honorary Doctorate Conferees". University of Haifa.
  7. ^Swerts, Bquadrats - Josi."EFPA News: Aristotle Prize 2011 awarded to Professor Marinus van IJzendoorn - Leiden University".
  8. ^"Bowlby-Ainsworth Awards"(PDF).www.bischof.com. 2011.
  9. ^"Dr. Hendrik Muller Prize for researcher Rien van IJzendoorn". Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. 28 June 2013. Retrieved27 April 2016.
  10. ^"Association for Psychological Science: APS Fellows".
  11. ^"Highly Cited Researchers - the Most Influential Scientific Minds". Archived fromthe original on 5 December 2016. Retrieved14 February 2017.
  12. ^"Eregalerij 2016". 23 December 2016.
  13. ^(in Dutch)Certificate, personal website, retrieved 1 July 2017.
  14. ^"Lolle Nauta Foundation » Welcome".
  15. ^"Prof.dr. (Marinus) M.H. Van IJzendoorn".
  16. ^Bowlby (1969). Attachment and Loss: I Attachment, Hogarth Press: London. pp. 371.[clarification needed]
  17. ^Verhage, M. L., Schuengel, C., Madigan, S., Fearon, R. M., Oosterman, M., Cassibba, R., ... & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment.
  18. ^Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (Eds.). (2012).Promoting positive parenting: An attachment-based intervention. Routledge.
  19. ^Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process: A social learning approach.Eugene, OR: Castalia.
  20. ^"Video-feedback Intervention to Promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP-SD) - Effectieve jeugdinterventies - NJi".
  21. ^Pereira, M.; Negrão, M.; Soares, I.; Mesman, J. (2014). "Decreasing harsh discipline in mothers at risk for maltreatment: A randomized control trial".Infant Mental Health Journal.35 (6):604–613.doi:10.1002/imhj.21464.PMID 25798509.
  22. ^Negrao, M.; Pereira, M.; Soares, I.; Mesman, J. (2014). "Enhancing positive parent-child interactions and family functioning in a poverty sample: a randomized control trial".Attachment & Human Development.16 (4):315–328.doi:10.1080/14616734.2014.912485.hdl:1822/86710.PMID 24972101.S2CID 9419167.
  23. ^Yagmur, S.; Mesman, J.; Malda, M.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Ekmekci, H. (2014). "Video-feedback intervention increases sensitive parenting in ethnic minority mothers: a randomized control trial".Attachment & Human Development.16 (4):371–386.doi:10.1080/14616734.2014.912489.PMID 24972105.S2CID 27640181.
  24. ^Stein, A.; Woolley, H.; Senior, R.; Hertzmann, L.; Lovel, M.; Lee, J.; Fairburn, C.G. (2006). "Treating disturbances in the relationship between mothers with bulimic eating disorders and their infants: a randomized, controlled trial of video feedback".The American Journal of Psychiatry.163 (5):899–906.doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.5.899.PMID 16648333.
  25. ^Juffer, F.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2009). "Attachment-based interventions: heading for evidence-based ways to support families".ACAMH Occasional Papers.29:47–60.
  26. ^Belsky, J.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J.; Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). "For better and for worse differential susceptibility to environmental influences".Current Directions in Psychological Science.16 (6):300–304.CiteSeerX 10.1.1.533.6364.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00525.x.S2CID 7782544.
  27. ^Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Pijlman, F.T.A.; Mesman, J.; Juffer, F. (2008). "Differential susceptibility to intervention: Dopamine D4 Receptor Polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates effects on toddlers' externalizing behavior in a randomized control trial".Developmental Psychology.44 (1):293–300.doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.293.PMID 18194028.
  28. ^Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Mesman, J.; Alink, L.R.A.; Juffer, F. (2008). "Effects of an attachment-based intervention on daily cortisol moderated by DRD4: A randomized control trial on 1-3-year-olds screened for externalizing behavior".Development and Psychopathology.20 (3):805–820.doi:10.1017/S0954579408000382.PMID 18606032.S2CID 702142.
  29. ^Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2015)."The hidden efficacy of interventions: Gene x Environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective".Annual Review of Psychology.66:381–409.doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015407.PMID 25148854.
  30. ^Van IJzendoorn, M. H.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2012)."A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group".Psychoneuroendocrinology.37 (3):438–443.doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.008.PMID 21802859.S2CID 14766597.
  31. ^Euser, E.M.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Prinzie, P.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2010). "The prevalence of child maltreatment in the Netherlands".Child Maltreatment.15 (1):5–17.doi:10.1177/1077559509345904.PMID 19729577.S2CID 22080912.
  32. ^Euser, S.; Alink, L.R.A.; Pannebakker, F.; Vogels, T.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2013). "The prevalence of child maltreatment in the Netherlands across a 5-year period".Child Abuse & Neglect.37 (10):841–851.doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.07.004.PMID 23938018.S2CID 21110156.
  33. ^Euser, S.; Alink, L.R.A.; Tharner, A.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2014). "Out of home placement to promote safety? The prevalence of physical abuse in residential and foster care".Children and Youth Services Review.37:64–70.doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2013.12.002.S2CID 72067754.
  34. ^Euser, S.; Alink, L.R.A.; Tharner, A.; Van IJzendoorn, M.H.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2013). "The prevalence of child sexual abuse in out-of-home care: A comparison between abuse in residential and in foster care".Child Maltreatment.18 (4):221–231.doi:10.1177/1077559513489848.PMID 23671258.S2CID 36168460.
  35. ^Euser, S., Alink, L.R.A., Tharner, A., van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2015). The prevalence of child sexual abuse in out-of-home care: Increased risk for children with a mild intellectual disability.Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 1-10.
  36. ^Sagi-Schwartz, A.; Van IJzendoorn, M. H.; Grossmann, K. E.; Joels, T.; Grossmann, K.; Scharf, M.; Alkalay, S. (2003). "Attachment and traumatic stress in female Holocaust child survivors and their daughters".American Journal of Psychiatry.160 (6):1086–1092.CiteSeerX 10.1.1.605.6306.doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1086.PMID 12777266.
  37. ^Sagi-Schwartz, A.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J.; Linn, S.; Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2013)."Against All Odds: Genocidal Trauma is Associated with Longer Life-Expectancy of the Survivors".PLOS ONE.8 (7): 7.Bibcode:2013PLoSO...869179S.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069179.PMC 3722177.PMID 23894427.

External links

edit

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp