Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Wikipedia

Keres language

(Redirected fromKeresan languages)
"Keresan" redirects here. For the US Navy cargo ship, seeUSS Keresan (ID-1806).
Not to be confused withKerek language.
This article shouldspecify the language of its non-English content, using{{langx}},{{transliteration}} for transliterated languages, and{{IPA}} for phonetic transcriptions, with an appropriateISO 639 code. Wikipedia'smultilingual support templates may also be used - notablymul for Multiple languages.See why.(September 2024)

Keres (/ˈkrs/),[2] alsoKeresan (/ˈkɛrəsən/), is aNative American language, spoken by theKeresPueblo people inNew Mexico. Depending on the analysis, Keres is considered a smalllanguage family or alanguage isolate with severaldialects. If it is considered a language isolate, it would be the most widely spoken language isolate within the borders of theUnited States. The varieties of each of the seven Keres pueblos aremutually intelligible with its closest neighbors. There are significant differences between the Western and Eastern groups, which are sometimes counted as separate languages.

Keresan
Native toUnited States
RegionNew Mexico
EthnicityKeres
Native speakers
13,190 (2013)[1]
Dialects
  • East Keres
  • West Keres
Language codes
ISO 639-3Either:
kee – Eastern
kjq – Western
Glottologkere1287
ELP
Pre-contact distribution of Keresan languages
Acoma-Laguna is classified as Definitely Endangered by theUNESCOAtlas of the World's Languages in Danger

Classification

edit

Keres is now considered alanguage isolate. In the past,Edward Sapir grouped it together with aHokan–Siouan stock.Morris Swadesh suggested a connection withWichita.Joseph Greenberg grouped Keres withSiouan,Yuchi,Caddoan, andIroquoian in a superstock called Keresiouan. None of these proposals has been validated by subsequent linguistic research.

Internal classification

edit

In 2007, there was an estimate total of 10,670 speakers.[3]

  • Keres
    • Eastern Keres: total of 4,580 speakers (1990 census)
    • Western Keres: total of 3,391 speakers (1990 census)

Phonology

edit

Keresan has between 42 and 45 consonant sounds, and around 40 vowel sounds, adding up to a total of about 85phonemes, depending on the analysis and the language variety. Based on the classification in theWorld Atlas of Language Structures, Keres is a language with alarge consonant inventory.

The great number of consonants relates to the three-way distinction betweenvoiceless,aspirated andejective consonants (e.g. /t tʰ tʼ/), and to the larger than average[4] number offricatives (i.e. /s sʼ ʂ ʂʼ ʃ ʃʼ h/) andaffricates, the latter also showing the three-way distinction found instops.

The large number of vowels derives from a distinction made betweenlong and short vowels (e.g. /e eː/), as well as from the presence oftones and voicelessness. Thus, a single vowel quality may occur with seven distinct realizations: /é è e̥ éː èː êː ěː/, all of which are used to distinguish words in the language.

Consonants

edit

The chart below contains the consonants of theproto-Keresan (or pre-Keresan) fromMiller & Davis (1963) based on a comparison of Acoma, Santa Ana, and Santo Domingo, as well as other features of the dialects compiled fromThe Language of Santa Ana Pueblo (1964),Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics (1987), andThe Phonemes of Keresan (1946), and theGrammar of Laguna Keres (2005).[5][6][7][8]

LabialAlveolarPalatalRetroflexVelarGlottal
Nasalvoicedmnɲ
glottalizedɲˀ
Plosivevoicelessptckʔ
aspirated
ejective
Affricatevoicelessts
aspiratedtsʰtʃʰtʂʰ
ejectivetsʼtʃʼtʂʼ
Fricativevoicelesssʃʂh
ejectiveʃʼʂʼ
Approximantvoicedwɾj
glottalizedɾˀ

Vowels

edit

Keresan vowels have aphonemic distinction induration: all vowels can be long or short. Additionally, short vowels can also be voiceless. The vowel chart below contains the vowel phonemes and allophones from the information of the Keresan languages combined fromThe Language of Santa Ana Pueblo (1964),[5]The Phonemes of Keresan (1946),[7] and Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics (1987).[6]

LongShort
PhonemicPhoneticPhonemicPhoneticVoiceless
Close/iː/[i]/i/[i ɪ][ɪ̥]
Mid-front/eː/[eː]/e/[e ɛ æ][e̥]
Mid-central/ɨː/[əː ɨː]/ɨ/[ə ɨ ɤ][ɨ̥]
Open/ɑː/[aː ɑː]/ɑ/[a ɑ][ḁ]
Back-close/oː/[oː]/o/[o][o̥]
/uː/[uː]/u/[u ʊ o][ʊ̥]

Notes:

  • Western Keres does not have phonemic /oː/ or /o/, though both vowels may occur phonetically.[8] Eastern Keres words containing /o/ show /au/ in Western Keres.[9] For instance, the first vowel in the word-sentenceSraúka̠cha̠ – “I see you”:
    • Kotyit Keres: [ʂóːkʰɑ̥tʃʰɑ̥]
    • Kʼawaika Keres: [ʂɑ̌ukʰɑ̥tʃʰɑ̥]

Voiceless vowels

edit

All Keresan short vowels may bedevoiced in certain positions. The phonemic status of these vowels is controversial.[8] Maring (1967) considers them to be phonemes of Áákʼu Keres, whereas other authors disagree. There are phonetic grounds for vowel devoicing based on the environment they occur, for instance word-finally, but there are also exceptions. Vowels in final position are nearly always voiceless and medial vowels occurring between voiced consonants, after nasals and ejectives are nearly always voiced.[10]

  • Word-final devoicing:[pɑ̌ːkʊ̥]because
  • Word-medial devoicing:[ʔìpʰi̥ʃɑ́]white paint

Tones

edit

Acoma Keres has fourlexical tones: high, low, falling and rising.[10] Falling and rising tones only occur in long vowels and voiceless vowels bear no tones:

Tonesexamplestranslation
High[tɨ́j], [áwáʔáwá]here, matrilineal uncle
Low[mùːtètsá]young boy
Rising[pɑ̌ːkʊ̥]because
Falling[ʔêː], [hêːk'a]and, whole part

Syllable structure

edit

Most Keresan syllables take a CV(V) shape.[8] The maximal syllable structure is CCVVC and the minimal syllable is CV. In native Keresan words, only a glottal stop /ʔ/ ⟨ʼ⟩ can close a syllable, but some loanwords from Spanish have syllables that end in a consonant, mostly a nasal (i.e. /m n/ but words containing these sequences are rare in the language.[11]

Syllable typeexamplestranslation
CV[sʼà], [ʔɪ]shv́vI have it, left
CVV[mùː]dedza, a[táù]shiyoung boy,cooking pot
CCV[ʃkʰí]srátsʼaI'm not fat
CCVV[ʃtùː]srabluejay
CVCí[miʔ], [kùm]banêeruexpression of fear, workmate (Spanish"compañero")

Due to extensive vowel devoicing, several Keresan words may be perceived as ending in consonants or even containing consonant clusters.

  • Word-internal cluster:yʼâakạ srûunị ‘stomach’ /jˀɑ̂ːkḁʂûːni/ > [jɑ̂ːkḁʂûːni] ~ [jɑ̂ːûːni]
  • Word-final coda:úwàakạ ‘baby’; /úwɑ̀ːkḁ/ > [úwɑ̀ːkʰḁ] ~ [úwɑ̀ː]

Phonotactics

edit

The only sequence of consonants (i.e.consonant cluster) that occurs in native Keresan words is a sequence of a fricative /ʃ ʂ/ and a stop or affricate. Clusters are restricted to beginnings of syllables (i.e. thesyllable onset). When the alveolo-palatal consonant /ʃ/ occurs as C1, it combines with alveolar and palatal C2, whereas the retroflex alveolar /ʂ/ precedes bilabial and velar C2s, which suggest a complementary distribution. Consonant clusters may occur both word-initially and word-medially.[9]

C1/C2BilabialAlveolarVelarPostalveolar
/p//pʰ//pʼ//t//tʰ//tʼ//k//kʰ//kʼ//tʃ//tʃʰ//tʃʼ/
/ʃ//ʃtáʊ̯rákʊ̥/

shdáurákụ

'frog, toad'

/ʃtʰéràʃtʼíká/

shtérashtʼígá

'cricket'

/ʃtʼìcɑ̀ːtʰɪ̥ʃɪ̥/

shtʼidyàatịshị

'plot of land'

/ʃtʃɨ/

shjv

'upward'

/ʃtʃʰúmúná/

shchúmúmá

'wasp'

/ʃtʃʼísḁ/

shchʼísạ

'six'

/ʂ//ʂpúːná/

srbúuná

'water jug'

/ʂpʰɑ̀ːtʼi/

srpàat'i

'mockingbird'

/ʂpʼeruru/

srpʼeruru

'it's full'

/ʂkɑ́ʂkɑ́ʊ̯kʼa/

srgásrgáukʼa

'quail'

/ʂkʰɨ́tútsʰɪ̥/

srkv́dútsị

'mound, hill'

/ʂkʼàpɪ́hɪ́/

srkʼabíhí

'female in-law'

Orthography

edit

Traditional Keresan beliefs postulate that Keres is a sacred language that must exist only in its spoken form.[12] The language's religious connotation and years of persecution of Pueblo religion by European colonizers may also explain why no unified orthographic convention exists for Keresan. However, a practical spelling system has been developed for Laguna (Kʼawaika)[8] and more recently for Acoma (Áakʼu) Keres,[13] both of which are remarkably consistent.

In the Keres spelling system, each symbol represents a single phoneme. The letters ⟨c q z f⟩ and sometimes also ⟨v⟩ are not used.Digraphs represent both palatal consonants (written using a sequence of C and ⟨y⟩), and retroflex consonants, which are represented using a sequence of C and the letter ⟨r⟩. Thesegraphemes used for writing Western Keres are shown between ⟨...⟩ below.

Consonant symbols

edit
LabialAlveolarPalatalRetroflexVelarGlottal
Nasalvoiced⟨m⟩⟨n⟩⟨ny⟩
glottalized⟨mʼ⟩⟨nʼ⟩⟨nyʼ⟩
Plosivevoiceless⟨b⟩⟨d⟩⟨dy⟩⟨g⟩⟨ʼ⟩
aspirated⟨p⟩⟨t⟩⟨ty⟩⟨k⟩
ejective⟨pʼ⟩⟨tʼ⟩⟨tyʼ⟩⟨kʼ⟩
Affricatevoiceless⟨dz⟩⟨j⟩⟨dr⟩
aspirated⟨ts⟩⟨ch⟩⟨tr⟩
ejective⟨tsʼ⟩⟨chʼ⟩⟨trʼ⟩
Fricativevoiceless⟨s⟩⟨sh⟩⟨sr⟩⟨h⟩
ejective⟨sʼ⟩⟨shʼ⟩⟨srʼ⟩
Approximantvoiced⟨w⟩⟨r⟩⟨y⟩
glottalized⟨wʼ⟩⟨rʼ⟩⟨yʼ⟩

Signage at Acoma Pueblo

edit

Signs at Acoma Pueblo sometimes use special diacritics for ejective consonants that differ from the symbols above, as shown in the table:

 
Signage at Acoma Pueblo
Comparison between general orthpgraphy and the orthography used at Acoma
General⟨pʼ⟩⟨tʼ⟩⟨kʼ⟩⟨sʼ⟩⟨tsʼ⟩⟨mʼ⟩⟨wʼ⟩⟨yʼ⟩⟨nʼ shʼ srʼ tyʼ⟩
Acoma signage⟨ṕ⟩⟨t́⟩⟨ḱ⟩⟨ś⟩⟨tś⟩⟨ḿ⟩⟨ẃ⟩⟨ý⟩?


Vowel symbols

edit

Vowel sounds are represented straightforwardly in the existing spellings for Keresan. Each vowel sound is written using a unique letter or digraph (for long vowels anddiphthongs). However, there are two competing representations for the vowel /ɨ/. Some versions simply use theIPA ⟨ɨ⟩ whereas others use the letter ⟨v⟩ (the sound /v/ as inveal does not occur in Keresan). Voiceless vowels have also been represented in two ways; either underlined or with a dot below (see table).

Long vowelsShort vowelsVoiceless vowels
PhonemeGraphemePhonemeGraphemePhonemeGrapheme
/ iː /⟨ii⟩/ i /⟨i⟩/ ɪ̥ /⟨i̱⟩ or ⟨ị⟩
/ eː /⟨ee⟩/ e /⟨e⟩/ e̥ /⟨e̱⟩ or ⟨ẹ⟩
/ ɨː /⟨ɨɨ⟩ or ⟨vv⟩/ ɨ /⟨ɨ⟩ or ⟨v⟩/ ɨ̥ /⟨ɨ̱⟩ or ⟨ṿ⟩
/ ɑː /⟨aa⟩/ ɑ /⟨a⟩/ ḁ /⟨a̱⟩ or ⟨ạ⟩
/ oː /⟨oo⟩/ o /⟨o⟩/ o̥ /⟨o̱⟩ or ⟨ọ⟩
/ uː /⟨uu⟩/ u /⟨u⟩/ ʊ̥ /⟨u̱⟩ or ⟨ụ⟩

Diacritics for tone

edit

Tone may or may not be represented in the orthography of Keresan. When represented, four diacritics may be used above the vowel. Unlike the system used forNavajo, diacritics for tone are not repeated in long vowels.

High toneLow toneRising toneFalling tone
Long Vowel⟨áa⟩, ⟨úu⟩⟨àa⟩, ⟨ùu⟩ or unmarked⟨ǎa⟩, ⟨ǔu⟩ or ⟨aá⟩, ⟨uú⟩⟨âa⟩, ⟨ûu⟩ or ⟨aà⟩, ⟨uù⟩
Short Vowel⟨á⟩, ⟨ú⟩⟨à⟩, ⟨ù⟩ or unmarked-

Keres orthography and alphabetical order

edit

Although Keresan is not normally written, there exists one dictionary of the language in which words are listed in any given order. In thisdictionary of Western Keres,digraphs count as single letters, although ejective consonants are not listed separately; occurring after their non-ejective counterparts. The symbol for the glottal stop ⟨ʼ⟩, for long vowels (e.g. ⟨aa ee ii⟩ etc.) are not treated as separate letters.

Alphabetical order in the Acoma Keres Audio Dictionary
A aB bCH chCHʼ chʼD dDR drDY dyDZ dzE eG gH hI i(Ɨ ɨ)J jK kKʼ kʼ
M mMʼ mʼN nNʼ nʼNY nyNYʼ nyʼ(O o)P pPʼ pʼR rRʼ rʼS sSʼ sʼSH shSHʼ shʼSR sr
SRʼ srʼT tʼTR trTRʼ trʼTS tsTSʼ tsʼTY tyTYʼ tyʼU uW w(V v)Wʼ wʼY yYʼ yʼ

Letters〈f q x z〉are not used to write Keres, whereas the letters ⟨ɨ o v⟩ are only used in some dialects.


Sample texts

edit

Orthography marking tone

edit
Woodpecker and Coyote[9]
⟨Ái dítʼîishu srbígà kʼánâaya dyáʼâʼu. Shʼée srbígà ái dyěitsị ái náyáa shdyɨ dyáʼa.⟩
/ ɑ́ì títʼîːʃù ʂpíkɑ̀ kʼɑ́nɑ̂ːjɑ̀ cɑ́ʔɑ̂ʔù | ʃʼéː ʂpíkɑ̀ ɑ́ì cěǐtsʰi̥ ɑ́ì nɑ́jɑ́ː ʃcɨ̀ cɑ́ʔɑ̀ /

Orthography without tone marking

edit
Boas text[8]
Baanaʼa, egu kauʼseeʼe, atsi sʼaama-ee srayutse.

Morphosyntax

edit

Keresan is asplit-ergative language in which verbs denoting states (i.e.stative verbs) behave differently from those indexing actions, especially in terms of thepersonaffixes they take. This system ofargument marking is based on a split-intransitive pattern, in whichsubjects are marked differently if they are perceived as actors than from when they are perceived as undergoers of the action being described.

The morphology of Keresan is mostlyprefixing, althoughsuffixes andreduplication also occur.[9] Keresan distinguishesnouns, verbs,numerals andparticles as word classes. Nouns in Keresan do not normally distinguishcase ornumber, but they can beinflected forpossession, with distinct constructions foralienable and inalienable possession. Other than possession, Keresan nouns show no comprehensivenoun classes.

Word order

edit

Keresan is averb-final language, though word order is rather flexible.[9][8]

Negation is doubly marked in Keresan. In addition to the adverbdzaadi, verbs index negation through a suffix (e.g.-u).

  • Gukacha 'S/he saw her/him'
  • Dzaadi gukachau 'S/he didn't see her/him'

Verbal morphology

edit

Theverb is a centralgrammatical category in Keres, conveying the most information about events in communicative acts.[8][9][10] Through itsmorphemes, Keresan verbs code not only person and number of the initiator of the action (e.g. “Tammy drinks decaf”) as is common inIndo-European languages, but also how the initiator is implicated in the action. For instance, the three verbs that describe Tammy's actions in “Tammy kicked the ball” vs. “Tammy jumped” vs. “Tammy sneezed” require different levels of effort from Tammy, that is whenkicking vs.jumping vs.sneezing.

Additionally, the person and number of the undergoer of the action are all coded on the verb (e.g. the wordgukacha means “S/he sees her/him”, a full sentence in English). The ways the speaker assesses the action (i.e.evidentiality, as in “I think Tammy arrived from class” vs. “Tammy arrived from class”). Finally, the internal temporal structure of the action (i.e.aspect, as in “Tammywas sneezing in class” vs. “Tammy sneezed in class”) is also coded in Keresan verbs.

According to Maring (1967), the Keresan verb is organized around the following grammatical categories (pp. 39–40)[10]

  • Subject/Object relations
    • Subject of intransitive verbs: marked by a prefix that distinguishes 3-4 persons in the singular (see below).
    • Subject of transitive verbs: marked by a prefix that distinguishes 3-4 persons in the singular (see below).
    • Object of transitive verbs: marked by a prefix that combines with the subject prefix, or by asuffix
  • Number relations
    • Singular: usually marked by a prefix
    • Dual: can be marked by a prefix, partial reduplication or by suffixes
    • Plural: can be marked by a prefix, partial reduplication, by suffixes or bysuppletive stem forms (i.e. singular and plural forms are not relatedetymologically)
  • Temporal relations
    • Future: is marked on the verb by a series of prefixes that also encode number
  • Modality relations
  • Voice relations
  • Aspect
    • Imperfective
    • Inceptive
    • Repetitive
    • Continuative
    • Habitual
    • Inchoative
    • Perfective

The verbal prefix

edit

In Keres, the verbal prefix carries information from five different grammatical categories:argument role,modality,polarity,[8] person and number. That is, a single Keresan verb prefix codes who initiated the action and how implicated that entity is (the subject/case), whom underwent the effects of the action (the direct object), the speaker's assessment of the action (the modality)[14] and whether it occurred or not (polarity). On the other hand, information about when the action took place (i.e.tense) is expressed elsewhere in a clause, mostly by adverbs.[9]

Number
edit

Keresan verbs distinguish three numbers:singular,dual (two entities) andplural (more than two entities); and fourpersons:first (the speaker), second (the hearer), third (a known, definite or salient entity being talked about) andfourth (a non-salient, unknown or indefinite entity being talked about, also known as obviative) persons. The plural and dual forms are often marked by reduplication of part of the stem (gukacha ‘s/he saw it’ vsguʼukacha ‘the two of them saw it’).

Argument role
edit

Languages encode two main types of actions: those in which the main participant initiates an action that produces change in an object (e.g.kicka ball, buya gift, cooka dish, reada book); and those in which the action produces no (perceived) change in the world or that have no object (sneezing, breathing, growing, diving, etc.).[15] Actions that take an object are encoded bytransitive verbs, whereas those that take no object are expressed viaintransitive verbs.

Intransitive verbs
edit

InIndo-European languages like English, all intransitive verbs behave similarly (‘They sneeze/breathe/dive/think’/etc.). In Keresan, actions that take no object are conceptualized in two distinct ways depending on how the initiator of the action is implicated. More active-like intransitive verbs (e.g. ‘to sneeze’) are coded through one set of morphemes, whereas actions conceptualized as involving the initiator at a lesser degree (e.g. ‘to believe’) are coded using a separate set of prefixes.

Degrees of involvement of the initiator in Keres[8]
ActionsIntransitive verb type
Moreto write (-dyàatra),to steal as a thief (-chʼáwʼa),to have diarrhea (-ushchʼi),

to leave (-mi),to whistle (-srbiitsa),to sweat (-shdyuwàan’i)

Active
Lessto believe (-hima),to be born (-dyá),to sleep (-bái),

to be afraid (-tyishu),to forget (-dyúmidruwi)

Inactive

Ideas expressed in Indo-European languages with adjectives are most often encoded by verbs in Keresan. That is, in Keresan one express the idea in the sentence ‘He is selfish’ by saying something along the lines of ‘Heselfishes’. In such “actions”, the entity that is characterized by them is not implicated in the action directly (i.e. it's beyond their control), and thus belong in the Inactive intransitive category. The different sets of prefixes are shown below:

Intransitive Prefixes by Verb Type
Active intransiveInactive intransitive
PrefixExamplePrefixExample
Firsts(i)-sudyàatraI writesrk-srkuhimaI believe
Secondsr-srúuchʼáwʼayou stealkɨdr-kɨdrâidyáyou were born
Thirdk-kashdyuwàanʼis/he sweatsdz-dzíibáihe is sleeping
Transitive verbs
edit
Transitive verb - Indicative mood (-ukạchạ 'to see')
Direct object
Singular
SubjectFirst ('me’)Second (‘you’)Third (‘her’/‘him’)Fourth
First

(‘I’)

-srà-ukạchạsì-ukạchạ-
I see youI see her/him
Second

(‘you’)

dyù-ukạchạ-srù-ukạchạ
you see meyou see her/him
Third

(‘she’/‘he’)

srgù-ukạchạkudrù -ukạchạg-ukạchạgù-ukạchạ
s/he sees mes/he sees yous/he sees her/hims/he sees something
Fourth

(‘one’)

-dzì-ukạchạ-
one sees it

Aspect

edit

Aspect in Keresan is signalled by suffixes.

-ajanu 'to rain'
kájániit rains
káajániit is raining
kájásɨit keeps raining
káajatúit rained

Time (tense) adverbials

edit

The category of tense is expressed in Keresan via adverbs that indicate when the action about which one is speaking took place.

Time adverbials in Acoma Keres[10]
PastFuture
tsikʼínumalong agokúsratonight
hámaonce, formerlynachámatomorrow
súwayesterdaynaháayashiday after tomorrow

Lexicon

edit

New words are coined through a number of roots that are combined to pre-existing ones. Compounding is a common strategy for word building, althoughderivation also occurs.

Numerals

edit

The Keresan numeral system is a base 10 system. Numerals 11–19, as well as those between the multiple of tens, are formed by adding the wordkʼátsi (/ kʼátsʰɪ / 'ten')followed by the worddzidra (/tsɪtʂa/ 'more'). Numerals 20 and above are formed by adding a multiplicative adverb (-wa or-ya) to the base number and the wordkʼátsi.[8]

Western Keres
1ísrkʼé11kʼátsi-írskʼá-dzidra21dyúya-kʼátsi-íisrkʼé-dzidra
2dyúuwʼée12kʼátsi-dyú-dzidra22dyúya-kʼátsi-dyú-dzidra
3chameʼée13kʼátsi-chami-dzidra30chamiya-kʼátsi
4dyáana14kʼátsi-dyáana-dzidra40dyáanawa-kʼátsi
5táam'a15kʼátsi-táamʼa-dzidra50táamʼawa-kʼátsi
6shʼísa16kʼátsi-shchʼísa-dzidra60shchʼísawa-kʼátsi
7mʼáiʼdyàana17kʼátsi-mʼáidyana-dzidra70mʼáidyanawa-kʼátsi
8kukʼúmishu18kʼátsi-kukʼúmishu-dzidra80kukʼúmishuwa-kʼátsi
9máyúkʼu19kʼátsi-máiyúkʼa-dzidra90máiyúkʼuwa-kʼátsi
10kʼátsi20dyúwa-kʼátsi100kʼádzawa-kʼátsi

Loanwords from Spanish

edit

European colonizers arriving in theSouthwest US brought with themmaterial culture and concepts that were unknown to the peoples living in the area. Words for the new ideas introduced by Spaniards were oftenborrowed into Keres directly fromEarly Modern Spanish, and a large number of these persists in Modern Keresan.[11]

Semantic domainModern Western KeresModern SpanishEnglish translation
Household itemskamárîita, kuchâaru, kujûuna, méesa, mendâan, kuwêetacamarita, cuchara, colchón, mesa, ventana, cubeta (Mexico)bed, spoon, mattress, table, window (glass), bucket
Social structuregumbanêerụ, rái, murâatụ, merigâanạ, kumanirá, ninêerucompañero, rey, mulato, americano(a), comunidad, dineroworkmate, king, black person, white person, community house, money
Foodgéesu, arûusị, kawé, kurántụ, mantạgîiyụ, mandêegạqueso, arroz, café, cilantro, mantequilla, mantecacheese, rice, coffee, cilantro, butter, lard/butter
Animal husbandrykawâayu, kanêeru, kujíinu, kurá, dûura, wáakạshịcaballo, carnero, cochino, corral, toro, vacahorse, sheep, pen/corral, bull, cow
Religious conceptsmíisa, Háasus Kuríistị, nachạwêena, guréesimamisa, Jesús Cristo, Noche Buena, Cuaresmamass, Jesus Christ, Christmas, Lent
Days of the weektamîikụ, rûunishị, mâatịsị, mérikụsị, sruwêewesị, yêenịsị, sâawarudomingo, lunes, martes, miércoles, jueves, viernes, sábadoSunday, Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday, Friday, Saturday

Proto-language

edit
Proto-Keresan
Reconstruction ofKeresan languages

Selected Proto-Keresan reconstructions of plants, animals, and toponyms by Miller and Davis (1963)[16] are given below. The IPA transcription is based on the authors' description. Underlined consonants in reconstructions refer to uncertainties by the authors regarding aspiration (p. 312); these are shown as capitalized consonants in the IPA.

no.glossProto-KeresanIPA
10wheat*ʔáṣánɪ*/ʔáʂáni̥/
17centipede*ʔíʔìˑdʸawa*/ʔíʔìːcàwà/
19cholla cactus*ʔiˑbánɪ*/ʔiːpáni̥/
27porcupine*ʔiˑṣ̍á*/ʔiːʂʼá/
45toad*bêˑrak̠ᴀ*/pêːɾàKḁ/
63turkey*cinᴀ*/tsʰinḁ/
64fox*cúsk̠ɪ*/tsʰúsKi̥/
71locust*c̍íˑga*/tsʼíːkà/
72Zia Pueblo*c̍íˑy̍á*/tsʼíːjˀá/
78kiva*c̆ídʸá*/tʃícá/
83medicine man*č̇áyâˑni*/tʃʼájâːnì/
84hawk*č̇ɨ́ˑríga*/tʃʼɨ́ːɾíka/
85horned toad*dabínᴜsk̠ᴀ*/*tapínu̥sKḁ/
87Santa Ana Pueblo*dámáyá*/támájá/
88squash*dâˑni*/tâːnì/
91corn husk*díˑskámí*/tíːskʰámí/
93dog*díyᴀ/*tíjḁ/
98bobcat*dʸáˑdʸᴜ*/cáːcu̥/
101deer*dʸán̍é*/cánˀé/
104gourd*dʸáˑwí*/cáːwí/
105piñon pine*dʸèic̠ɪ*/cèiTSi̥
108elk*dʸɨ́ˑṣᴀ*/cɨ́ːʂḁ/
110badger*dʸúˑbí*/*cúːpí/
112beans*gánami*/kánàmì/
114seed*gáwɪc̠ɪ*/káwi̥TSi̥/
119bear*gúháyᴀ*/kúhájḁ/
124yucca*háʔásc̐á*/háʔástʃʼá/
127oak*ha̍ˑbánɪ
137pine tree*hâˑniˑ*/hâːniː/
147Jemez Pueblo*héˑmíšíˑ-cɪ, *héˑmíšíˑ-zé/*héːmíʃíː-tsʰi̥/, */*héːmíʃíː-tsé/
149turtle*héyᴀdʸɪ*/héjḁci̥/
157willow*híẓᵻsk̍áwa*/*híʂɨ̥skʼáwa/
158dove*húˑʔùˑga
161yucca fruit*hùˑsk̍ani
169antelope*kɨ́ˑc̠ɪ
175wolf*k̍ákana
176spider*k̍ámᴀsk̠ᵻ
198mountain lion*mûˑk̍aiẓᴀ
200buffalo*múšêiẓᴀ
201soapweed*múšɪ
213hummingbird*m̍îˑzᴀ
225prairie dog*nɨ́t̠ɪ
232bedbug*peséc̍uru
239salamander*p̍águra
241rabbit*rèˑdʸᴀ
246woodpecker*sbíga
247chicken*sbíˑná
251meadowlark*sc̐áˑná
254grasshopper*sc̐ár̍ɪ
260crow*sc̐ɨ́r̍á
262wild honey*sc̐úmᵻ
264mosquito*sc̐úy̍úˑná
274ant*síˑʔí
275squirrel*síˑdʸᴀ
279mouse*síyan̍ᵻ
282bighorn sheep*skàˑsk̠ᴜ
286bullsnake*sk̍áʔáˑdʸᴜ
287fish*sk̍àˑšᵻ
291peas*sk̍úrúˑná
293dwarf corn*spíníní
306parrot*šâˑwit̠ᴀ
307flea, louse*šínaˑ
309goose*šúˑdá
318blue jay*ṣúisɪ
319snake*ṣûˑwiˑ
342abalone shell*w̍a̍ˑbɨ́nɪ
347duck*w̍âˑyuṣᴀ
354corn silk*yábášɪ
355corn*yáˑčínɪ
356mesquite*yêˑt̠ᴜ
357worm*yúʔúbɨ́
369corn cob*y̍úˑskúm̍á

In popular media

edit

Keres was one of the seven languages sung in the Coca-Cola "It's Beautiful" commercial during the2014 Super Bowl featuring "America the Beautiful".[17]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^"Detailed Languages Spoken at Home and Ability to Speak English for the Population 5 Years and over: 2009-2013".
  2. ^"Keres".Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary. Merriam-Webster.
  3. ^"Keres language, alphabet, and pronunciation".Omniglot. RetrievedApril 28, 2021.
  4. ^Ian., Maddieson (1984).Patterns of sounds. Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]: Cambridge University Press.ISBN 9780521113267.OCLC 10724704.
  5. ^abDavis, Irvine (1964).The Language of Santa Ana Pueblo, Smithsonian Bulletin 191, Anthropological Papers, No. 69.
  6. ^abA Comparative Sketch of Pueblo Languages: Phonology. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics. 1987.
  7. ^abSpencer, Robert F. (1946).The Phonemes of Keresan.
  8. ^abcdefghijklLachler, Jordan (2005).Grammar of Laguna Keres. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Dissertation.
  9. ^abcdefgValiquette, Hilaire (1990).A study for a lexicon of Laguna Keresan.
  10. ^abcdeMaring, Joel M. (1967).Grammar of Acoma Keresan. Indiana University Dissertation.
  11. ^abSpencer, Robert (1947). "Spanish Loanwords in Keresan".Southwestern Journal of Anthropology.3 (2):130–146.doi:10.1086/soutjanth.3.2.3628729.S2CID 164169397.
  12. ^Brandt, Elizabeth (1981). "Native American Attitudes toward Literacy and Recording in the Southwest".Journal of the Linguistic Association of the Southwest.4 (2):185–195.
  13. ^"The Keres Language Project".The Keres Language Project. Retrieved2018-03-18.
  14. ^L., Bybee, Joan (1994).The evolution of grammar : tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Perkins, Revere D. (Revere Dale), Pagliuca, William. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.ISBN 0226086631.OCLC 29387125.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^Givón, Talmy (2001).Syntax : an introduction. Volume 1 (Rev. ed.). Amsterdam: J. Benjamins.ISBN 1588110656.OCLC 70727915.
  16. ^Miller, Wick R.; Davis, Irvine (1963)."Proto-Keresan Phonology".International Journal of American Linguistics.29 (4):310–330.doi:10.1086/464748.S2CID 143519987.
  17. ^"Native Language Spotlighted During Coca-Cola Super Bowl Ad".Indian Country Today Media Network. 2014-02-03. Archived fromthe original on 2014-03-01. Retrieved2014-02-26.

Bibliography

edit

External links

edit
Wiktionary has a list of reconstructed forms atAppendix:Proto-Keresan reconstructions

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp