Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


[Skip to Navigation]
Our website uses cookies to enhance your experience. By continuing to use our site, or clicking "Continue," you are agreeing to ourCookie Policy | Continue
JAMA Network Home
JAMA Psychiatry
    Sign In
    Individual Sign In
    Sign inCreate an Account
    Access through your institution
    Sign In
    Visual Abstract. Pain Reprocessing Therapy for Chronic Back Pain
    Pain Reprocessing Therapy for Chronic Back Pain
    Figure 1.  CONSORT Participant Flow Diagram
    CONSORT Participant Flow Diagram

    MRI indicates magnetic resonance imaging.

    Figure 2.  Clinical Outcomes
    Clinical Outcomes

    A, Shading indicates standard error. B, Dots represent individual participants; thick lines represent the group mean. C, Percentage of patients reporting pain scores of 0 or 1 of 10 (ie, pain-free or nearly pain-free) at posttreatment and at 1-year follow-up. PRT indicates pain reprocessing therapy.

    Figure 3.  Effects of Treatment on Evoked and Spontaneous Back Pain and Related Brain Function
    Effects of Treatment on Evoked and Spontaneous Back Pain and Related Brain Function

    A, Error bars show standard error. B, Coordinates and statistics for activations provided in eTable 7 inSupplement 2; analyses conducted within a mask of regions of interest; eFigure 1 inSupplement 2. C, Decreased evoked pain-related activity was observed in anterior midcingulate (aMCC) and anterior prefrontal regions for PRT vs placebo and left anterior insula for PRT vs usual care. D, Error bars show standard error. E, PRT vs control conditions increased aPFC-seeded (red clusters) and aIns-seeded (green clusters) connectivity with primary somatosensory cortex (permutation test,P < .05). Inset shows seed regions, derived from evoked pain analyses; magenta outlines, PRT vs placebo; black outlines, PRT vs usual care. F, PRT vs usual care increased connectivity between an aMCC seed (yellow; derived from evoked back pain analyses) and the precuneus (orange). Connectivity analyses were conducted within primary somatosensory cortex and medial default mode network masks.

    aP < .001.

    bP < .05.

    Table 1.  Baseline Patient Characteristics
    Baseline Patient Characteristics
    CharacteristicNo. (%)
    Pain reprocessing therapyPlaceboUsual care
    Demographic characteristics
    Age, mean (SD), y42.6 (16.2)39.4 (14.9)41.3 (15.9)
    Sex
    Female29 (58)25 (49)27 (54)
    Male21 (42)26 (51)23 (46)
    Education
    High school or less000
    Some college11 (22)15 (29)15 (30)
    College graduate39 (78)36 (71)35 (70)
    Married26 (52)25 (49)30 (60)
    Racea
    American Indian or Alaskan Native001 (2)
    Asian/Pacific Islander3 (6)2 (4)0
    Black (not of Hispanic origin)02 (4)1 (2)
    White (not of Hispanic origin)46 (92)45 (88)43 (86)
    Other or unknown1 (2)2 (4)5 (10)
    Hispanic ethnicity02 (4)2 (4)
    Employment status
    Full-time (>30 h/wk)33 (66)26 (51)28 (56)
    Part-time (5-30 h/wk)10 (20)12 (24)13 (26)
    Unemployed/lightly employed (<5 h/wk)7 (14)13 (25)9 (18)
    Subjective socioeconomic status, mean (SD), 1-106.8 (1.8)6.4 (2.0)6.7 (1.6)
    Exercise
    Almost none6 (12)1 (2)4 (8)
    1 h/wk4 (8)7 (14)9 (18)
    3 h/wk17 (34)23 (45)14 (28)
    7 h/wk19 (38)18 (35)21 (42)
    ≥14 h/wk4 (8)2 (4)2 (4)
    Pain-related characteristics
    Pain duration, mean (SD), y10.7 (9.7)8.9 (8.2)10.5 (8.9)
    Current opioid use (yes/no)5 (10)2 (4)2 (4)
    Pain in body sites besides back?
    None5 (10)9 (18)4 (8)
    A little29 (58)24 (47)28 (56)
    A moderate amount11 (22)15 (29)16 (32)
    A lot5 (10)3 (6)2 (4)
    Table 2.  Primary and Secondary Clinical Outcomes
    Primary and Secondary Clinical Outcomes
    Between-group differencesaMean (SD)PRT vs placebo,g (SE)bP valuePRT vs usual care,g (SE)bP value
    PRTPlaceboUsual care
    Primary outcome
    Pain intensity (0-10)
    Baseline4.22 (1.21)4.16 (1.33)3.91 (1.24)NANANANA
    Posttreatment1.18 (1.24)2.84 (1.64)3.13 (1.45)−1.14 (0.24)<.001−1.75 (0.24)<.001
    At 1 mo1.26 (1.77)2.91 (1.97)3.07 (1.63)−0.83 (0.27)<.001−1.24 (0.29)<.001
    At 2 mo1.59 (1.92)3.06 (1.89)3.00 (1.86)−0.84 (0.28).001−1.03 (0.28)<.001
    At 3 mo1.54 (1.68)3.21 (2.02)3.27 (1.95)−0.93 (0.23)<.001−1.35 (0.25)<.001
    At 6 mo1.39 (1.48)2.68 (2.08)2.95 (1.93)−0.74 (0.23).001−1.14 (0.26)<.001
    At 12 mo1.51 (1.59)2.79 (1.78)3.00 (1.77)−0.70 (0.21).001−1.05 (0.24)<.001
    Secondary outcome
    Oswestry Disability Index (0-100)
    Baseline23.70 (10.70)23.06 (10.14)23.26 (9.67)NANANANA
    Posttreatment10.14 (10.63)19.00 (11.07)20.68 (10.68)−1.30 (0.28)<.001−1.70 (0.26)<.001
    At 1 mo10.58 (14.26)18.68 (11.95)20.30 (9.04)−1.04 (0.25)<.001−1.61 (0.27)<.001
    At 2 mo9.57 (12.86)19.43 (11.84)21.37 (11.07)−1.30 (0.29)<.001−1.55 (0.23)<.001
    At 3 mo9.68 (13.39)21.42 (14.32)23.57 (13.36)−1.26 (0.28)<.001−1.61 (0.25)<.001
    At 6 mo9.80 (11.94)18.50 (13.43)20.84 (11.57)−0.96 (0.26)<.001−1.3 (0.28)<.001
    At 12 mo11.16 (13.13)18.52 (12.60)18.78 (12.59)−0.23<.001−0.83 (0.24)<.001
    PROMIS depression, raw score (8-32)
    Baseline14.66 (4.39)13.17 (4.67)12.85 (4.74)NANANANA
    Posttreatment12.23 (4.94)11.75 (4.05)11.81 (4.45)−0.35 (0.24).099−0.56 (0.24).009
    At 1 mo12.87 (5.23)10.64 (3.57)11.57 (4.61)0.13 (0.23).555−0.54 (0.25).019
    At 2 mo12.51 (4.88)11.11 (4.95)11.76 (5.17)−0.08 (0.24).723−0.51 (0.24).028
    At 3 mo11.47 (4.64)12.45 (6.09)12.30 (4.51)−0.57 (0.24).015−0.90 (0.22)<.001
    At 6 mo12.90 (5.28)10.97 (4.00)11.84 (4.65)−0.09 (0.24).701−0.47 (0.23)0.40
    At 12 mo12.53 (5.12)11.95 (5.86)12.75 (4.50)−0.20 (0.23).360−0.62 (0.24).007
    PROMIS anger, raw score (5-25)
    Baseline12.46 (3.73)10.97 (3.18)11.17 (3.18)NANANANA
    Posttreatment9.52 (3.91)9.89 (3.81)10.45 (3.86)−0.62 (0.21).004−0.78 (0.21)<.001
    At 1 mo9.50 (4.40)8.84 (3.27)10.55 (3.19)−0.23 (0.25).291−0.91 (0.25)<.001
    At 2 mo10.70 (4.68)9.37 (3.30)10.00 (3.92)−0.11 (0.23).652−0.28 (0.25).231
    At 3 mo9.31 (4.06)9.87 (4.78)10.49 (3.52)−0.52 (0.21).027−0.92 (0.25)<.001
    At 6 mo9.83 (4.49)9.31 (2.96)10.51 (3.44)−0.38 (0.25).099−0.90 (0.23)<.001
    At 12 mo10.49 (4.15)9.64 (3.55)10.89 (3.38)−0.16 (0.21).454−0.61 (0.22).008
    PROMIS anxiety, raw scores (8-40)
    Baseline16.37 (5.88)15.52 (5.83)15.11 (6.40)NANANANA
    Posttreatment15.02 (6.16)13.89 (5.78)14.11 (6.99)0 (0.22)1.00−0.21 (0.21).318
    At 1 mo14.58 (6.45)12.25 (4.81)13.75 (6.78)0.36 (0.20).109−0.29 (0.21).203
    At 2 mo14.14 (7.07)13.23 (6.74)13.58 (6.75)0.02 (0.24).923−0.22 (0.25).348
    At 3 mo13.75 (6.45)14.50 (7.42)14.08 (6.42)−0.34 (0.23).147−0.62 (0.21).009
    At 6 mo14.88 (7.12)13.00 (5.14)14.59 (6.90)0.03 (0.24).907−0.50 (0.24).028
    At 12 mo14.09 (6.79)14.07 (7.51)14.81 (6.94)−0.20 (0.22).362−0.56 (0.23).014
    PROMIS sleep, raw score (8-40)
    Baseline22.21 (6.54)22.65 (6.38)22.63 (6.26)NANANANA
    Posttreatment17.73 (6.75)20.50 (6.17)20.89 (6.02)−0.41 (0.23).056−0.63 (0.22).003
    At 1 mo17.18 (6.38)21.02 (6.34)21.62 (6.45)−0.46 (0.25).039−0.89 (0.27)<.001
    At 2 mo17.08 (6.71)19.71 (6.72)21.74 (7.19)−0.38 (0.24).112−0.84 (0.27)<.001
    At 3 mo16.67 (6.67)20.16 (7.05)21.73 (6.26)−0.44 (0.24).061−1.08 (0.24)<.001
    At 6 mo17.85 (7.24)19.42 (6.22)21.38 (6.03)−0.29 (0.23).198−0.85 (0.23)<.001
    At 12 mo18.11 (7.36)19.95 (5.79)21.19 (6.73)−0.23 (0.22).272−0.60 (0.25).009
    Supplement 2.

    eMethods

    eResults

    eDiscussion

    eTable 1. Spinal anomalies among participants randomized to PRT

    eTable 2. Treatment response rates

    eTable 3. Secondary clinical outcomes measured only at pretreatment and posttreatment

    eTable 4. Treatment satisfaction and patient global impression of change

    eTable 5. Mediation results

    eTable 6. Values for mediators at each timepoint

    eTable 7. Evoked back pain localizer results

    eTable 8. Regions showing pretreatment to posttreatment connectivity changes for PRT vs placebo or PRT vs usual care

    eFigure 1. Evoked back pain localizer

    eFigure 2. Target masks for seed connectivity analyses

    eFigure 3. Individual trajectories of pain intensity for participants in the PRT, placebo and usual care groups

    eFigure 4. Effects of PRT on pain-related fear and avoidance and beliefs that pain indicates injury

    eFigure 5. Evoked back pain at pretreatment

    eFigure 6. High vs low thumb pressure stimulation

    eFigure 7. Histogram of quality control-functional connectivity correlations for spontaneous pain scans

    eFigure 8. Continuous pain regressors for 4 randomly chosen sample individuals

    eAppendix 1. Initial medical pain assessment and education session

    eAppendix 2. Pain reprocessing therapy description

    eAppendix 3. PRT treatment fidelity checklist

    eReferences

    1.
    Dahlhamer  J, Lucas  J, Zelaya  C,  et al.  Prevalence of chronic pain and high-impact chronic pain among adults—United States, 2016.  MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2018;67(36):1001-1006. doi:10.15585/mmwr.mm6736a2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    2.
    Institute of Medicine. Relieving Pain in America: A Blueprint for Transforming Prevention, Care, Education, and Research. National Academies Press; 2011. doi:10.17226/13172
    3.
    Hartvigsen  J, Hancock  MJ, Kongsted  A,  et al; Lancet Low Back Pain Series Working Group.  What low back pain is and why we need to pay attention.  Lancet. 2018;391(10137):2356-2367. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30480-XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    4.
    Maher  C, Underwood  M, Buchbinder  R.  Non-specific low back pain.  Lancet. 2017;389(10070):736-747. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30970-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    5.
    Vlaeyen  JWS, Maher  CG, Wiech  K,  et al  Low back pain.  Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2018;4(1):52. doi:10.1038/s41572-018-0052-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    6.
    Henschke  N, Maher  CG, Refshauge  KM,  et al.  Prevalence of and screening for serious spinal pathology in patients presenting to primary care settings with acute low back pain.  Arthritis Rheum. 2009;60(10):3072-3080. doi:10.1002/art.24853PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    7.
    Deyo  RA, Weinstein  JN.  Low back pain.  N Engl J Med. 2001;344(5):363-370. doi:10.1056/NEJM200102013440508PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    8.
    Karshikoff  B, Jensen  KB, Kosek  E,  et al  Why sickness hurts: a central mechanism for pain induced by peripheral inflammation.  Brain Behav Immun. 2016;57:38-46. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2016.04.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    9.
    Nicholas  M, Vlaeyen  JWS, Rief  W,  et al; IASP Taskforce for the Classification of Chronic Pain.  The IASP classification of chronic pain for ICD-11: chronic primary pain.  Pain. 2019;160(1):28-37. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001390PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    10.
    Clauw  DJ.  Diagnosing and treating chronic musculoskeletal pain based on the underlying mechanism(s).  Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol. 2015;29(1):6-19. doi:10.1016/j.berh.2015.04.024PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    11.
    Williams  ACC, Fisher  E, Hearn  L, Eccleston  C.  Psychological therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults.  Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;8(8):CD007407. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD007407.pub4PubMedGoogle Scholar
    12.
    Chou  R, Deyo  R, Friedly  J,  et al.  Nonpharmacologic therapies for low back pain: a systematic review for an American College of physicians clinical practice guideline.  Ann Intern Med. 2017;166(7):493-505. doi:10.7326/M16-2459PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    13.
    Woolf  CJ.  Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain.  Pain. 2011;152(3)(suppl):S2-S15. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    14.
    Kuner  R, Flor  H.  Structural plasticity and reorganisation in chronic pain.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;18(1):20-30. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.162PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    15.
    Corder  G, Ahanonu  B, Grewe  BF, Wang  D, Schnitzer  MJ, Scherrer  G.  An amygdalar neural ensemble that encodes the unpleasantness of pain.  Science. 2019;363(6424):276-281. doi:10.1126/science.aap8586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    16.
    Dale  J, Zhou  H, Zhang  Q,  et al.  Scaling up cortical control inhibits pain.  Cell Rep. 2018;23(5):1301-1313. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2018.03.139PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    17.
    Hua  T, Chen  B, Lu  D,  et al.  General anesthetics activate a potent central pain-suppression circuit in the amygdala.  Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(7):854-868. doi:10.1038/s41593-020-0632-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    18.
    Kleckner  IR, Zhang  J, Touroutoglou  A,  et al.  Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans.  Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1:0069. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0069PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    19.
    Barrett  LF.  The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(1):1-23. doi:10.1093/scan/nsx060PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    20.
    Barrett  LF, Simmons  WK.  Interoceptive predictions in the brain.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(7):419-429. doi:10.1038/nrn3950PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    21.
    Sterling  P.  Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation.  Physiol Behav. 2012;106(1):5-15. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    22.
    Baliki  MN, Apkarian  AV.  Nociception, pain, negative moods, and behavior selection.  Neuron. 2015;87(3):474-491. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    23.
    Petzschner  FH, Weber  LAE, Gard  T, Stephan  KE.  Computational psychosomatics and computational psychiatry: toward a joint framework for differential diagnosis.  Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82(6):421-430. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.05.012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    24.
    Büchel  C, Geuter  S, Sprenger  C, Eippert  F.  Placebo analgesia: a predictive coding perspective.  Neuron. 2014;81(6):1223-1239. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    25.
    Tracey  I.  Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans.  Nat Med. 2010;16(11):1277-1283. doi:10.1038/nm.2229PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    26.
    Kaptchuk  TJ, Hemond  CC, Miller  FG.  Placebos in chronic pain: evidence, theory, ethics, and use in clinical practice.  BMJ. 2020;370:m1668. doi:10.1136/bmj.m1668PubMedGoogle Scholar
    27.
    Seymour  B.  Pain: a precision signal for reinforcement learning and control.  Neuron. 2019;101(6):1029-1041. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.055PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    28.
    Ashar  YK, Chang  LJ, Wager  TD.  Brain mechanisms of the placebo effect: an affective appraisal account.  Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13(1):73-98. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    29.
    Jepma  M, Koban  L, van Doorn  J, Jones  M, Wager  TD.  Behavioural and neural evidence for self-reinforcing expectancy effects on pain.  Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(11):838-855. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0455-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    30.
    Vlaeyen  JWS, Crombez  G.  Behavioral conceptualization and treatment of chronic pain.  Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2020;16:187-212. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095744PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    31.
    Meulders  A.  Fear in the context of pain: Lessons learned from 100 years of fear conditioning research.  Behav Res Ther. 2020;131:103635. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2020.103635PubMedGoogle Scholar
    32.
    Hashmi  JA, Baliki  MN, Huang  L,  et al.  Shape shifting pain: chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits.  Brain. 2013;136(Pt 9):2751-2768. doi:10.1093/brain/awt211PubMedGoogle Scholar
    33.
    Napadow  V, Harris  RE.  What has functional connectivity and chemical neuroimaging in fibromyalgia taught us about the mechanisms and management of ‘centralized’ pain?  Arthritis Res Ther. 2014;16(5):425. doi:10.1186/s13075-014-0425-0PubMedGoogle Scholar
    34.
    Roy  M, Shohamy  D, Daw  N, Jepma  M, Wimmer  GE, Wager  TD.  Representation of aversive prediction errors in the human periaqueductal gray.  Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(11):1607-1612. doi:10.1038/nn.3832PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    35.
    Čeko  M, Shir  Y, Ouellet  JA, Ware  MA, Stone  LS, Seminowicz  DA.  Partial recovery of abnormal insula and dorsolateral prefrontal connectivity to cognitive networks in chronic low back pain after treatment.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(6):2075-2092. doi:10.1002/hbm.22757PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    36.
    Martucci  KT, Shirer  WR, Bagarinao  E,  et al.  The posterior medial cortex in urologic chronic pelvic pain syndrome: detachment from default mode network—a resting-state study from the MAPP Research Network.  Pain. 2015;156(9):1755-1764. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000238PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    37.
    Woo  C-W, Roy  M, Buhle  JT, Wager  TD.  Distinct brain systems mediate the effects of nociceptive input and self-regulation on pain.  PLoS Biol. 2015;13(1):e1002036. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002036PubMedGoogle Scholar
    38.
    Kober  H, Buhle  J, Weber  J, Ochsner  KN, Wager  TD.  Let it be: mindful acceptance down-regulates pain and negative emotion.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(11):1147-1158. doi:10.1093/scan/nsz104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    39.
    Lutz  A, McFarlin  DR, Perlman  DM, Salomons  TV, Davidson  RJ.  Altered anterior insula activation during anticipation and experience of painful stimuli in expert meditators.  Neuroimage. 2013;64(1):538-546. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.030PubMedGoogle Scholar
    40.
    Xiao  L, Yank  V, Ma  J.  Algorithm for balancing both continuous and categorical covariates in randomized controlled trials.  Comput Methods Programs Biomed. 2012;108(3):1185-1190. doi:10.1016/j.cmpb.2012.06.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    41.
    Thorn  BE, Eyer  JC, Van Dyke  BP,  et al.  Literacy-adapted cognitive behavioral therapy versus education for chronic pain at low-income clinics: a randomized controlled trial.  Ann Intern Med. 2018;168(7):471-480. doi:10.7326/M17-0972PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    42.
    Vlaeyen  JWS, Morley  S, Linton  S, Boersma  K, De Jong  J. Pain-Related Fear: Exposure-Based Treatment for Chronic Pain. IASP Press; 2012.
    43.
    Moseley  GL, Butler  DS.  Fifteen years of explaining pain: the past, present, and future.  J Pain. 2015;16(9):807-813. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    44.
    McCracken  LM, Vowles  KE.  Acceptance and commitment therapy and mindfulness for chronic pain: model, process, and progress.  Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):178-187. doi:10.1037/a0035623PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    45.
    Lumley  MA, Schubiner  H, Lockhart  NA,  et al.  Emotional awareness and expression therapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, and education for fibromyalgia: a cluster-randomized controlled trial.  Pain. 2017;158(12):2354-2363. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001036PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    46.
    Cherkin  DC, Sherman  KJ, Balderson  BH,  et al.  Effect of mindfulness-based stress reduction vs cognitive behavioral therapy or usual care on back pain and functional limitations in adults with chronic low back pain: a randomized clinical trial.  JAMA. 2016;315(12):1240-1249. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.2323PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    47.
    Kabat-Zinn  J, Lipworth  L, Burney  R.  The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain.  J Behav Med. 1985;8(2):163-190. doi:10.1007/BF00845519PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    48.
    Garland  EL, Manusov  EG, Froeliger  B, Kelly  A, Williams  JM, Howard  MO.  Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for chronic pain and prescription opioid misuse: results from an early-stage randomized controlled trial.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 2014;82(3):448-459. doi:10.1037/a0035798PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    49.
    Kaptchuk  TJ, Friedlander  E, Kelley  JM,  et al.  Placebos without deception: a randomized controlled trial in irritable bowel syndrome.  PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e15591. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0015591PubMedGoogle Scholar
    50.
    Locher  C, Frey Nascimento  A, Kirsch  I, Kossowsky  J, Meyer  A, Gaab  J.  Is the rationale more important than deception? a randomized controlled trial of open-label placebo analgesia.  Pain. 2017;158(12):2320-2328. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    51.
    Kleine-Borgmann  J, Schmidt  K, Hellmann  A, Bingel  U.  Effects of open-label placebo on pain, functional disability, and spine mobility in patients with chronic back pain: a randomized controlled trial.  Pain. 2019;160(12):2891-2897. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001683PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    52.
    Lembo  A, Kelley  JM, Nee  J,  et al.  Open-label placebo vs double-blind placebo for irritable bowel syndrome: a randomized clinical trial.  Pain. 2021. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002234PubMedGoogle Scholar
    53.
    Schubiner  H, Betzold  M. Unlearn Your Pain. Mind Body Publishing; 2010.
    54.
    Napadow  V, Kim  J, Clauw  DJ, Harris  RE.  Decreased intrinsic brain connectivity is associated with reduced clinical pain in fibromyalgia.  Arthritis Rheum. 2012;64(7):2398-2403. doi:10.1002/art.34412PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    55.
    Mawla  I, Ichesco  E, Zöllner  HJ,  et al.  Greater Somatosensory Afference With Acupuncture Increases Primary Somatosensory Connectivity and Alleviates Fibromyalgia Pain via Insular γ-Aminobutyric Acid: A Randomized Neuroimaging Trial.  Arthritis Rheumatol. 2021;73(7):1318-1328. doi:10.1002/art.41620PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    56.
    Bushnell  MC, Ceko  M, Low  LA.  Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(7):502-511. doi:10.1038/nrn3516PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    57.
    Lazaridou  A, Kim  J, Cahalan  CM,  et al.  Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) on brain connectivity supporting catastrophizing in fibromyalgia.  Clin J Pain. 2017;33(3):215-221. doi:10.1097/AJP.0000000000000422PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    58.
    Kim  J, Mawla  I, Kong  J,  et al.  Somatotopically specific primary somatosensory connectivity to salience and default mode networks encodes clinical pain.  Pain. 2019;160(7):1594-1605. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001541PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    59.
    Kong  J, Spaeth  RB, Wey  H-Y,  et al.  S1 is associated with chronic low back pain: a functional and structural MRI study.  Mol Pain. 2013;9(1):43. doi:10.1186/1744-8069-9-43PubMedGoogle Scholar
    60.
    Adler  NE, Epel  ES, Castellazzo  G, Ickovics  JR.  Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women.  Health Psychol. 2000;19(6):586-592. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    61.
    Brinjikji  W, Luetmer  PH, Comstock  B,  et al.  Systematic literature review of imaging features of spinal degeneration in asymptomatic populations.  AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2015;36(4):811-816. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A4173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    62.
    Keefe  FJ, Caldwell  DS, Williams  DA,  et al  Pain coping skills training in the management of osteoarthritic knee pain: a comparative study.  Behav Ther. 1990;21(1):49-62. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80188-1Google ScholarCrossref
    63.
    Logan  DE, Carpino  EA, Chiang  G,  et al.  A day-hospital approach to treatment of pediatric complex regional pain syndrome: initial functional outcomes.  Clin J Pain. 2012;28(9):766-774. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182457619PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    64.
    Sherry  DD, Wallace  CA, Kelley  C, Kidder  M, Sapp  L.  Short- and long-term outcomes of children with complex regional pain syndrome type I treated with exercise therapy.  Clin J Pain. 1999;15(3):218-223. doi:10.1097/00002508-199909000-00009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    65.
    Craske  MG, Kircanski  K, Zelikowsky  M, Mystkowski  J, Chowdhury  N, Baker  A.  Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy.  Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(1):5-27. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    66.
    Barlow  DH, Craske  MG, Cerny  JA, Klosko  JS.  Behavioral treatment of panic disorder.  Behav Ther. 1989;20(2):261-282. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80073-5Google ScholarCrossref
    67.
    Seminowicz  DA, Moayedi  M.  The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in acute and chronic pain.  J Pain. 2017;18(9):1027-1035. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    68.
    Craig  AD.  How do you feel—now? the anterior insula and human awareness.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):59-70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    69.
    Lindquist  KA, Wager  TD, Kober  H, Bliss-Moreau  E, Barrett  LF.  The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review.  Behav Brain Sci. 2012;35(3):121-143. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000446PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    70.
    Geuter  S, Boll  S, Eippert  F, Büchel  C.  Functional dissociation of stimulus intensity encoding and predictive coding of pain in the insula.  Elife. 2017;6:e24770. doi:10.7554/eLife.24770PubMedGoogle Scholar
    71.
    Garland  EL, Gaylord  SA, Palsson  O, Faurot  K, Douglas Mann  J, Whitehead  WE.  Therapeutic mechanisms of a mindfulness-based treatment for IBS: effects on visceral sensitivity, catastrophizing, and affective processing of pain sensations.  J Behav Med. 2012;35(6):591-602. doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9391-zPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    72.
    Kim  J, Loggia  ML, Cahalan  CM,  et al.  The somatosensory link in fibromyalgia: functional connectivity of the primary somatosensory cortex is altered by sustained pain and is associated with clinical/autonomic dysfunction.  Arthritis Rheumatol. 2015;67(5):1395-1405. doi:10.1002/art.39043PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

    See More About


    Select Your Interests

    Select Your Interests

    Customize your JAMA Network experience by selecting one or more topics from the list below.

    • Academic Medicine
    • Acid Base, Electrolytes, Fluids
    • Allergy and Clinical Immunology
    • American Indian or Alaska Natives
    • Anesthesiology
    • Anticoagulation
    • Art and Images in Psychiatry
    • Assisted Reproduction
    • Bleeding and Transfusion
    • Cardiology
    • Caring for the Critically Ill Patient
    • Challenges in Clinical Electrocardiography
    • Climate and Health
    • Climate Change
    • Clinical Challenge
    • Clinical Implications of Basic Neuroscience
    • Clinical Pharmacy and Pharmacology
    • Coaching
    • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
    • Consensus Statements
    • Coronavirus (COVID-19)
    • Critical Care Medicine
    • Cultural Competency
    • Dental Medicine
    • Dermatology
    • Diabetes and Endocrinology
    • Diagnostic Test Interpretation
    • Digital Health
    • Drug Development
    • Emergency Medicine
    • End of Life, Hospice, Palliative Care
    • Environmental Health
    • Equity, Diversity, and Inclusion
    • Ethics
    • Facial Plastic Surgery
    • Gastroenterology and Hepatology
    • Genetics and Genomics
    • Genomics and Precision Health
    • Geriatrics
    • Global Health
    • Guide to Statistics and Methods
    • Guidelines
    • Hair Disorders
    • Health Care Delivery Models
    • Health Care Economics, Insurance, Payment
    • Health Care Quality
    • Health Care Reform
    • Health Care Safety
    • Health Care Workforce
    • Health Disparities
    • Health Inequities
    • Health Policy
    • Health Systems Science
    • Hematology
    • History of Medicine
    • Humanities
    • Hypertension
    • Images in Neurology
    • Implementation Science
    • Infectious Diseases
    • Innovations in Health Care Delivery
    • JAMA Forum
    • JAMA Infographic
    • Law and Medicine
    • Leading Change
    • Less is More
    • LGBTQIA Medicine
    • Lifestyle Behaviors
    • Medical Coding
    • Medical Devices and Equipment
    • Medical Education
    • Medical Education and Training
    • Medical Journals and Publishing
    • Melanoma
    • Narrative Medicine
    • Nephrology
    • Neurology
    • Neuroscience and Psychiatry
    • Notable Notes
    • Nursing
    • Nutrition
    • Nutrition, Obesity, Exercise
    • Obesity
    • Obstetrics and Gynecology
    • Occupational Health
    • Oncology
    • Ophthalmology
    • Orthopedics
    • Otolaryngology
    • Pain Medicine
    • Palliative Care
    • Pathology and Laboratory Medicine
    • Patient Care
    • Patient Information
    • Pediatrics
    • Performance Improvement
    • Performance Measures
    • Perioperative Care and Consultation
    • Pharmacoeconomics
    • Pharmacoepidemiology
    • Pharmacogenetics
    • Pharmacy and Clinical Pharmacology
    • Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
    • Physical Therapy
    • Physician Leadership
    • Poetry
    • Population Health
    • Primary Care
    • Professional Well-being
    • Professionalism
    • Psychiatry and Behavioral Health
    • Public Health
    • Pulmonary Medicine
    • Radiology
    • Regulatory Agencies
    • Reproductive Health
    • Research, Methods, Statistics
    • Resuscitation
    • Rheumatology
    • Risk Management
    • Scientific Discovery and the Future of Medicine
    • Sexual Health
    • Shared Decision Making and Communication
    • Sleep Medicine
    • Sports Medicine
    • Stem Cell Transplantation
    • Substance Use and Addiction Medicine
    • Surgery
    • Surgical Innovation
    • Surgical Pearls
    • Teachable Moment
    • The Art of JAMA
    • The Arts and Medicine
    • The Rational Clinical Examination
    • Tobacco and e-Cigarettes
    • Toxicology
    • Translational Medicine
    • Trauma and Injury
    • Treatment Adherence
    • Ultrasonography
    • Urology
    • Users' Guide to the Medical Literature
    • Vaccination
    • Venous Thromboembolism
    • Veterans Health
    • Violence
    • Women's Health
    • Workflow and Process
    • Wound Care, Infection, Healing
    Save Preferences

    Others Also Liked

    2 Comments for this article
    EXPAND ALL
    September 21, 2022
    Methodologic and Other Concerns
    Lars-Petter Granan, MD, PhD | Department of Pain Management and Research, Oslo University Hospital, Norway
    I have several concerns about this study:

    1. A study inclusion criterion was primary back pain score at least 4/10 on a numerical rating scale for average pain intensity over the last week. 192 patients did not meet inclusion criteria due to too low a pain score and an additional 24 were excluded for the same reason when assessed in person. Nevertheless, in table 2 the authors report that baseline mean pain intensity was 4.22, 4.16 and 3.91 in the different patient groups, i.e., two very low average scores compared to the inclusion criterion and one below the inclusion
    criterion. eFigure3 suggests individual patients with a baseline score below 4/10 were included.

    2. Supplement 2 eTable3 suggests that cannabis use in the PRT group was more than twice that of the placebo group (7.5 vs 3.9 g) and seven times higher than in the usual care group (7.5 vs 1.06 g), differences of potential importance that should be discussed, as observed effects of PRT might be influenced by “co-treatments” such as cannabis.

    3. Related, if PRT is so effective, why did patients continue their use of cannabis (mean 6.6 g post-treatment) and opioids (mean 1.29 g post-treatment) at levels higher than the other groups? Shouldn’t that be unnecessary? 

    4. The article was accepted for publication July 27 and published online September 29 2021. The book mentioned in the conflict of interest disclosure, “The Way Out,” was released August 24 2021 with the subtitle “A Revolutionary, Scientifically Proven Approach to Healing Chronic Pain”. The science behind this book is the article I question.

    CONFLICT OF INTEREST:None Reported
    READ MORE
    October 13, 2022
    Reply to Dr. Granan from the authors
    Yoni Ashar, PhD | University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus
    As the authors of this article, we thank Dr. Granan for these comments and for the opportunity to address these important points.

    1. We wish to address the concern about baseline pain values by further explaining study procedures. We first screened for patients with at least 4 of 10 pain using an online pre-screening form (excluding 192 potential participants, see Figure 1 CONSORT Flow Diagram). Following the online pre-screen, potentially eligible participants were invited for an in-person eligibility and informed consent session, where we also required at least 4 of 10 pain for trial enrollment (excluding another 24 whose
    last-week average pain was below 4). We additionally assessed back pain intensity at the pre-randomization fMRI session, but no participants were dropped from the trial at this point for less than 4 of 10 pain intensity. As we reported at the end of the Clinical Measures section (p. 16 of the print version), baseline pain was computed as the average score from two pre-randomization, post-informed consent timepoints—the eligibility session and pretreatment fMRI session—to increase measurement reliability for baseline pain. Consistent with most clinical trials, our sample had some overall reduction in pain between the in-person eligibility session and the pretreatment fMRI assessment session, reducing the baseline values (an average of these two timepoints) to closer to 4 of 10, and less than 4 of 10 for some patients.

    2. Cannabis and opioid use in this trial was concentrated in a relatively small number of participants, with median values of 0 g / 0 pills in all groups, and a few participants reporting relatively high use. As such, we think these factors are unlikely to have a large influence on results. Here, we stratified randomization on age, gender, opioid use, and baseline pain (see Participants and Trial Design section, p. 14 in print version). Future studies could additionally stratify randomization on cannabis use or other concurrent treatments, though feasibility concerns limit the number of stratification covariates. Yet, unmeasured factors can always differ between trial arms—a main motivation for random assignment.

    3. Cannabis and opioid use did not change from pre- to post-treatment as this was not targeted by PRT, which is a relatively brief intervention focused on pain reduction. Participants may have also been using these substances for reasons other than pain management, in which case no change in use would necessarily be expected.

    4. The book mentioned aims to provide an accessible, self-guided format to the themes and techniques that are the focus of the treatment we studied here. It was written by author A.G. in parallel to, but separate from, the clinical trial. The authors primarily responsible for the scientific direction of the study and assessment of outcomes (Y.A. and T.D.W.) had no vested interest, financial or otherwise, in the book.
    CONFLICT OF INTEREST:First author of the article in question, consultant with Lin Health, Inc., Mental Health Partners of Boulder County, and the Pain Reprocessing Therapy Center
    READ MORE
    Original Investigation
    September 29, 2021

    Effect of Pain Reprocessing Therapy vs Placebo and Usual Care for Patients With Chronic Back Pain:A Randomized Clinical Trial

    Author AffiliationsArticle Information
    • 1Department of Psychiatry, Weill Cornell Medical College, New York City, New York
    • 2Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado, Boulder
    • 3Institute of Cognitive Science, University of Colorado, Boulder
    • 4Pain Psychology Center, Los Angeles, California
    • 5Ascension Providence Hospital, Southfield, Michigan
    • 6Michigan State University College of Human Medicine, East Lansing
    • 7Panorama Orthopedics and Spine Center, Golden, Colorado
    • 8Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois
    • 9Department of Philosophy, Washington University in Saint Louis, Saint Louis, Missouri
    • 10Johns Hopkins University Department of Biostatistics, Baltimore, Maryland
    • 11Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts
    • 12Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia
    • 13Renée Crown Wellness Institute, University of Colorado, Boulder
    • 14Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan
    • 15Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire
    JAMA Psychiatry. 2022;79(1):13-23. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.2669
    Key Points

    Question Can a psychological treatment based on the reappraisal of primary chronic back pain as due to nondangerous central nervous system processes provide substantial and durable pain relief?

    Findings In this randomized clinical trial, 33 of 50 participants (66%) randomized to 4 weeks of pain reprocessing therapy were pain-free or nearly pain-free at posttreatment, compared with 10 of 51 participants (20%) randomized to placebo and 5 of 50 participants (10%) randomized to usual care, with gains largely maintained through 1-year follow-up. Treatment effects on pain were mediated by reduced beliefs that pain indicates tissue damage, and longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging showed reduced prefrontal responses to evoked back pain and increased resting prefrontal-somatosensory connectivity in patients randomized to treatment relative to patients randomized to placebo or usual care.

    Meaning Psychological treatment focused on changing beliefs about the causes and threat value of primary chronic back pain may provide substantial and durable pain relief.

    Abstract

    Importance Chronic back pain (CBP) is a leading cause of disability, and treatment is often ineffective. Approximately 85% of cases are primary CBP, for which peripheral etiology cannot be identified, and maintenance factors include fear, avoidance, and beliefs that pain indicates injury.

    Objective To test whether a psychological treatment (pain reprocessing therapy [PRT]) aiming to shift patients’ beliefs about the causes and threat value of pain provides substantial and durable pain relief from primary CBP and to investigate treatment mechanisms.

    Design, Setting, and Participants This randomized clinical trial with longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and 1-year follow-up assessment was conducted in a university research setting from November 2017 to August 2018, with 1-year follow-up completed by November 2019. Clinical and fMRI data were analyzed from January 2019 to August 2020. The study compared PRT with an open-label placebo treatment and with usual care in a community sample.

    Interventions Participants randomized to PRT participated in 1 telehealth session with a physician and 8 psychological treatment sessions over 4 weeks. Treatment aimed to help patients reconceptualize their pain as due to nondangerous brain activity rather than peripheral tissue injury, using a combination of cognitive, somatic, and exposure-based techniques. Participants randomized to placebo received an open-label subcutaneous saline injection in the back; participants randomized to usual care continued their routine, ongoing care.

    Main Outcomes and Measures One-week mean back pain intensity score (0 to 10) at posttreatment, pain beliefs, and fMRI measures of evoked pain and resting connectivity.

    Results At baseline, 151 adults (54% female; mean [SD] age, 41.1 [15.6] years) reported mean (SD) pain of low to moderate severity (mean [SD] pain intensity, 4.10 [1.26] of 10; mean [SD] disability, 23.34 [10.12] of 100) and mean (SD) pain duration of 10.0 (8.9) years. Large group differences in pain were observed at posttreatment, with a mean (SD) pain score of 1.18 (1.24) in the PRT group, 2.84 (1.64) in the placebo group, and 3.13 (1.45) in the usual care group. Hedgesg was −1.14 for PRT vs placebo and −1.74 for PRT vs usual care (P < .001). Of 151 total participants, 33 of 50 participants (66%) randomized to PRT were pain-free or nearly pain-free at posttreatment (reporting a pain intensity score of 0 or 1 of 10), compared with 10 of 51 participants (20%) randomized to placebo and 5 of 50 participants (10%) randomized to usual care. Treatment effects were maintained at 1-year follow-up, with a mean (SD) pain score of 1.51 (1.59) in the PRT group, 2.79 (1.78) in the placebo group, and 3.00 (1.77) in the usual care group. Hedgesg was −0.70 for PRT vs placebo (P = .001) and −1.05 for PRT vs usual care (P < .001) at 1-year follow-up. Longitudinal fMRI showed (1) reduced responses to evoked back pain in the anterior midcingulate and the anterior prefrontal cortex for PRT vs placebo; (2) reduced responses in the anterior insula for PRT vs usual care; (3) increased resting connectivity from the anterior prefrontal cortex and the anterior insula to the primary somatosensory cortex for PRT vs both control groups; and (4) increased connectivity from the anterior midcingulate to the precuneus for PRT vs usual care.

    Conclusions and Relevance Psychological treatment centered on changing patients’ beliefs about the causes and threat value of pain may provide substantial and durable pain relief for people with CBP.

    Trial Registration ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier:NCT03294148.

    Introduction

    Chronic pain affects 20% of people in the US, with an estimated annual cost of more than $600 billion.1,2 The most common type is chronic back pain (CBP). In approximately 85% of cases, definitive peripheral causes of CBP cannot be identified, and central nervous system processes are thought to maintain pain.3-7 For people with this type of CBP— often referred to as primary, nonspecific, nociplastic, or centralized pain—psychological and behavioral treatments are recommended.8-10 Although these treatments can improve functioning, reductions in pain intensity are limited11,12 and better treatments are needed.

    Quiz Ref IDAdvances in the neuroscience of pain13-17 and interoception18-21 suggest new directions for treatment development. In constructionist and active inference models, pain is a prediction about bodily harm, shaped by sensory input and context-based predictions.18,19,22-26 Fearful appraisals of tissue damage can cause innocuous somatosensory input to be interpreted and experienced as painful.22,24,27,28 Such constructed perceptions can become self-reinforcing: threat appraisals enhance pain, which is in turn threatening, creating positive feedback loops that maintain pain after initial injuries have healed.27,29-31

    As pain becomes chronic, it is increasingly associated with activity in the affective and motivational systems tied to avoidance and less closely tied to systems encoding nociceptive input.14,32-34 Accordingly, brain regions serving allostasis and predictive control18,23—including the default mode network, somatosensory and insular cortices, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens—have been implicated in animal models13-17 and human studies of chronic pain22,25,32,33,35,36 and pain modulation.24,25,28,37-39

    We developed pain reprocessing therapy (PRT) based on this understanding of primary chronic pain. Leading psychological interventions for pain typically present the causes of pain as multifaceted and aim primarily to improve functioning and secondarily to reduce pain. PRT emphasizes that the brain actively constructs primary chronic pain in the absence of tissue damage and that reappraising the causes and threat value of pain can reduce or eliminate it.

    In this study, we conducted the first test of PRT. In a randomized clinical trial with 1-year follow-up, we compared PRT with both open-label placebo and usual care control conditions. We tested hypothesized mechanisms of PRT with mediation analyses and longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during spontaneously occurring and evoked back pain. fMRI provided objective correlates of treatment effects and identified potential neurobiological treatment mechanisms.

    Methods
    Participants and Trial Design

    The trial was preregistered on ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier:NCT03294148) and conducted from August 2017 to November 2018, with 1-year follow-up completed by November 2019. Clinical and fMRI data were analyzed from January 2019 to August 2020, after data collection at each follow-up timepoint was complete. Participants aged 21 to 70 years with back pain for at least half the days of the last 6 months and 1-week average pain intensity score of 4 of 10 or greater at screening were recruited from the community in Boulder, Colorado. We targeted primary CBP, excluding patients with leg pain worse than back pain (eMethods inSupplement 2). Power analysis targeted 80% power (α = .05) to detect a medium effect (d = 0.62) on pain intensity at the primary end point (eMethods inSupplement 2). Participants provided written informed consent as approved by the University of Colorado Institutional Review Board. The study followed the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) reporting guideline for social and psychological intervention trials.

    Participants completed an eligibility and consent session, followed by a baseline assessment session with fMRI. They were subsequently randomized to PRT, placebo, or usual care with equal probability, balancing on age, sex, baseline pain, and opioid use using an imbalance-minimization algorithm40 (eMethods inSupplement2). The primary end point (posttreatment fMRI session) occurred 1 month after the baseline fMRI. Participants completed online follow-up assessments at 1, 2, 3, 6, and 12 months after the primary end point (Figure 1).

    Interventions
    PRT

    PRT seeks to promote patients’ reconceptualization of primary (nociplastic) chronic pain as a brain-generated false alarm. PRT shares some concepts and techniques with existing treatments for pain41-48 and with the cognitive behavioral treatment of panic disorder.66

    Participants completed a 1-hour telehealth evaluation and education session with a physician (H.S.) assessing likely centralized vs peripheral contributions to pain, including a review of available preexisting spinal imaging. Assessment findings and centralized pain education were shared with the patient (eAppendix 1 inSupplement 2).

    Quiz Ref IDParticipants then completed 8 individual 1-hour therapy sessions with a therapist with extensive PRT experience (A.G. or C.U.) twice weekly for 4 weeks. Techniques included (1) providing personalized evidence for centralized pain; (2) guided reappraisal of pain sensations while seated and while engaging in feared postures or movements; (3) techniques addressing psychosocial threats (eg, difficult emotions) potentially amplifying pain; and (4) techniques to increase positive emotions and self-compassion. PRT followed the treatment protocol found in eAppendix 2 inSupplement 2.

    Treatment fidelity was assessed by independent raters coding audiorecordings of PRT sessions (eMethods and eAppendix 3 inSupplement 2). A mean (SD) of 4.93 (0.87) of 6 PRT elements were present in each session, and all sessions included at least 3 elements, indicating high treatment fidelity.

    Open-label Placebo Plus Usual Care

    Participants watched 2 videos describing how placebo treatments can powerfully relieve pain even when known to be inert (eg, they can automatically trigger the body’s natural healing response).49 A subcutaneous injection described as saline was administered by a physician (K.K.) at the site of greatest back pain during an empathic, validating clinical encounter at an orthopedic medical center. Open-label placebo treatments are as effective or nearly as effective as traditional (deceptive) placebos for CBP and other chronic symptoms when administered in this manner (eMethods inSupplement 2).50-52 Participants in this group were also asked to continue their ongoing care as usual and not start other new treatments until after the study period.

    Usual Care

    Participants in this group were given no additional treatment. They agreed to continue their ongoing care as usual and not start new treatments before the posttreatment assessment. After the posttreatment assessment, they were given a chronic pain workbook53 and access tohttp://www.unlearnyourpain.com.

    Clinical Measures

    The primary outcome was average pain over the last week on a numerical rating scale from 0 to 10 from the Brief Pain Inventory Short Form, assessed at the 1-month postbaseline session. We also calculated the proportion of participants reporting pain reduction of 30% or more, pain reduction of 50% or more, and a pain score of 0 or 1, indicating a pain-free or nearly pain-free state. Secondary outcomes included pain interference (Oswestry Disability Index); Patient-Reported Outcome Measurement Information System (PROMIS) short forms for depression, anxiety, anger, and sleep quality; and the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (measure details in the eMethods inSupplement 2).

    We considered 3 measures of pain beliefs as potential mediators: (1) the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia (TSK-11), assessing belief that pain indicates injury and fear of movement; (2) the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS); and (3) the Survey of Pain Attitudes Emotion subscale (SOPA-Emotion), assessing beliefs that stress and negative emotion increase pain. Adverse events were recorded when participants spontaneously reported them to study personnel. Baseline pain was computed as the average score from 2 prerandomization assessments (eligibility session and pretreatment fMRI session).

    Neuroimaging Measures

    Structural T1 and multiband blood oxygenation level–dependent functional imaging was conducted on a 3-T Siemens Prisma Fit MRI scanner with standard fMRI preprocessing (eMethods inSupplement 2). During fMRI, participants completed (1) an evoked back pain task with a series of randomly ordered trials distending the back to 1 of 4 intensity levels and (2) a spontaneous pain scan in which participants rested and rated ongoing pain once per minute (design details in the eMethods inSupplement 2; fMRI data quality measures shown in eFigures 6 and 7 inSupplement 2). Participants rated pain during scanning on a visual analog scale from 0 (no pain) to 100 (worst pain imaginable).

    Statistical Analyses

    Intent-to-treat analyses (including all randomized patients) were performed for the primary outcome with a mixed-effects model (fitlme, MATLAB 2020a), including 2 group × time interactions (PRT vs placebo × posttreatment vs pretreatment and PRT vs usual care × posttreatment vs pretreatment), covariates for age and sex, and a random intercept per participant. Treatment response rates for 30% or greater reduction in pain, 50% or greater reduction in pain, and a pain-free or nearly pain-free state at posttreatment and 1-year follow-up were based on all randomized patients; those missing data were considered nonresponders. For follow-up time points and secondary outcomes, we calculated Hedgesg for the PRT vs placebo and PRT vs usual care comparisons. Follow-up time points were analyzed individually, testing group differences in change from baseline to each time points. The placebo vs usual care comparison will be reported elsewhere.

    To investigate psychological treatment mechanisms, we (1) correlated pretreatment to posttreatment changes in pain intensity with pretreatment to posttreatment changes in pain beliefs (TSK-11, PCS, and SOPA-Emotion) within each group and (2) tested pretreatment to posttreatment changes in pain beliefs as mediators of treatment effects on pain at follow-up timepoints (1 through 12 months posttreatment), controlling for baseline pain. PRT vs placebo and PRT vs usual care were tested in separate models. We also tested the reverse: whether pretreatment to posttreatment pain reductions mediated treatment effects on pain beliefs at follow-up, controlling for baseline pain beliefs (eMethods inSupplement 2). Correlational and mediation analyses were not prespecified in the trial protocol.

    Evoked Back Pain Analyses

    An evoked back pain localizer identified brain regions positively associated with evoked back pain intensity at baseline. The localizer was conducted within a mask of regions of interest (medial prefrontal, posteromedial, insula, cingulate, and somatosensory cortices; amygdala; and nucleus accumbens; eMethods and eFigure 1 inSupplement 2; localizer task design in eFigure 8 inSupplement 2). We tested for treatment effects (group × time interactions) in the average activity of clusters positively associated with evoked back pain using a mixed-effects (random-effects) model, applying a 1-tailed threshold ofP < .05 owing to directional hypotheses that PRT would reduce activity in pain-positive clusters.

    Spontaneous Pain Connectivity Analyses

    Evoked pain analyses identified group × time interactions in the anterior insula, anterior midcingulate (aMCC), and a prefrontal region. We submitted these 3 regions as seeds to connectivity analyses in the spontaneous pain scan. We conducted permutation tests (threshold-free cluster-enhancement; eMethods inSupplement 2) testing for group × time interactions in connectivity between these seed regions and 2 areas most often demonstrating altered connectivity in chronic pain: (1) the midline default mode network, including the medial prefrontal and posteromedial cortex, and (2) primary somatosensory cortex (S1)36,54-59 (masks in eFigure 2 inSupplement 2).

    Results

    We randomized 151 participants (54% female; mean [SD] age, 41.1 [15.6] years; mean [SD] CBP duration, 10.0 [8.9] years). At baseline, patients reported low to moderate pain intensity scores (mean [SD], 4.10 [1.26]) to 4.41 [1.29]) and disability (mean [SD], 23.34 [10.12] on the Oswestry Disability Index), with similar pain and demographic characteristics across groups (Table 1).

    Of 50 participants randomized to PRT, 44 (88%) completed all treatment sessions and the posttreatment assessment. Five participants dropped out prior to initiating PRT and 1 had an unrelated medical emergency. Of 51 participants randomized to placebo, 44 (86%) received the treatment, all of whom completed the posttreatment assessment. Of the 50 participants randomized to usual care, 47 (94%) completed the posttreatment assessment (Figure 1).

    Twenty patients in the PRT group had preexisting spinal imaging, all of which showed at least 1 spinal anomaly (median of 4 findings per patient; eTable 1 inSupplement 2) assessed by a physician (H.S.) as not causal of pain (eMethods and eAppendix 1 inSupplement 2).61

    Clinical Outcomes

    Quiz Ref IDPatients randomized to PRT reported substantial reductions in pain intensity at posttreatment compared with both control groups, with a mean (SD) pain score of 1.18 (1.24) in the PRT group, 2.84 (1.64) in the placebo group, and 3.13 (1.45) in the usual care group (Figure 2;Table 2). Patients in the PRT group reported a pain reduction of 1.79 (on the 0 to 10 numerical rating scale) relative to placebo (t137.63 = 6.06;P < .001;g, −1.14; 95% CI, −1.65 to −0.71) and reported a pain reduction of 2.40 relative to the usual care group (t135.69 = 8.13;P < .001;g, −1.74; 95% CI, −2.28 to −1.32). A total of 33 of 50 patients randomized to PRT (66%), corresponding to 73% of the 45 patients who initiated PRT, were pain-free or nearly pain-free at posttreatment, compared with 10 of 51 patients (20%) in the placebo group and 5 of 50 patients (10%) in the usual care group. At 1-year follow-up, effects of PRT on pain remained large relative to both control groups, with a mean (SD) pain score of 1.51 (1.59) in the PRT group, 2.79 (1.78) in the placebo group, and 3.00 (1.77) in the usual care group. Hedgesg was −0.70 for PRT vs placebo (P = .001) and −1.05 for PRT vs usual care (P < .001) (Table 2; treatment response rates in eTable 2 inSupplement 2; individual patient pain trajectories in eFigure 3 inSupplement 2).

    Analyses of secondary outcomes at posttreatment revealed significant reductions in disability and anger for PRT vs both controls (g, −0.62 to −1.7;P < .005) and improvements in sleep (g, −0.56;P = .009) and depression (g, −0.63;P = .003) relative to usual care (Table 2). Treatment gains on secondary outcomes were largely maintained at 1-year follow-up (Table 2). Significant PRT vs control effects were observed at posttreatment for positive affect (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule;g for PRT vs placebo, 0.63,g for PRT vs usual care, 0.59;P < .005; eTable 3 inSupplement 2) but not for negative affect or alcohol, cannabis, or opioid use (eTable 3 inSupplement 2). Treatment satisfaction was high among participants in the PRT group (eTable 4 inSupplement 2).

    Mediation Analyses

    Pretreatment to posttreatment reductions in TSK-11 and pain intensity scores were correlated among participants in the PRT group (r42 = 0.44;P = .003; eFigure 4 inSupplement 2). This correlation was not significant for the placebo condition (r42 = 0.16;P = .29) or usual care condition (r45 = 0.27;P = .07). Pretreatment to posttreatment changes in PCS and SOPA-Emotion scores did not correlate with pain reductions within any group.

    Pretreatment to posttreatment reductions in TSK-11 scores mediated PRT vs placebo and PRT vs usual care effects on pain intensity at most follow-up time points (eFigure 4 and eTables 5 and 6 inSupplement 2). The reverse was also true: pretreatment to posttreatment pain reductions mediated PRT vs placebo and PRT vs usual care effects on TSK-11 at follow-up. Pretreatment to posttreatment changes in PCS and SOPA-Emotion did not mediate PRT vs control effects at any follow-up time point. Treatment effects on TSK-11 were very large at posttreatment (g for PRT vs placebo, −1.90;g for PRT vs usual care,−1.67;P < .001).

    Neither age nor sex moderated the treatment effect on pain intensity (eMethods inSupplement 2). No adverse events were reported for PRT.

    Neuroimaging Outcomes
    Evoked Back Pain

    At baseline, increased back distention led to increased pain (mean [SD] for distention level 1, 32.15 [18.57]; distention level 2, 37.91 [20.30]; distention level 3, 46.70 [21.71]; distention level 4, 52.73 [21.78]). There was a significant effect of distention level on pain (mean [SD] β for inflation, 7.05 [5.06];t95 = 13.64;P < .001. Individual patient-evoked pain data are shown in eFigure 5 inSupplement 2.

    Patients receiving PRT reported significant pretreatment to posttreatment reductions in evoked back pain relative to placebo (β, −13.05 on a 101-point visual analog scale;t122.85 = −2.82;P = .006;g, −0.60; 95% CI, −1.06 to −0.16) and relative to usual care (β, −19.61;t79.52 = −4.03;P < .001;g, −0.99; 95% CI, −1.50 to −0.55;Figure 3A). Among patients in the PRT group, pretreatment to posttreatment reductions in evoked back pain and 1-week average back pain intensity were correlated (r32 = 0.47;P = .005).

    Quiz Ref IDLocalizer analyses identified 16 regions within the mask of interest positively associated with evoked pain intensity, including bilateral insula, cingulate, bilateral somatotopic back areas S1 and secondary somatosensory cortex, and prefrontal regions (Figure 3B; eFigure 1 and eTable 7 inSupplement 2). Relative to placebo, PRT reduced pain-related activity in aMCC (t133.48 = −1.73;P = .04) and the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC;t133.48 = −1.85;P = .03). Relative to usual care, PRT reduced pain-related activity in the left anterior insula (aIns;t120.1 = −2.34;P = .01;Figure 3C).

    Spontaneous Pain

    Patients receiving PRT reported reductions in spontaneous pain relative to placebo (β, −18.24 on a 101-point visual analog scale;t140.66 = −4.59;P < .001;g, −0.92; 95% CI, −1.44 to −0.47) and relative to usual care (β, −21.53;t79 = −5.26;P < .001;g, −1.11; 95% CI, −1.66 to −0.66;Figure 3D).

    We submitted the aMCC, aPFC, and aIns regions exhibiting treatment effects in evoked pain analyses as connectivity seed regions in the spontaneous pain task. Within S1, PRT vs placebo and PRT vs usual care led to increased aPFC- and aIns-seeded connectivity to 4 distinct S1 subregions (permutation test COPE-MAX, 3.55-3.91;P < .05). Within the medial default mode network, PRT vs usual care increased aMCC-precuneus connectivity (permutation test COPE-MAX, 4.23;P = .01;Figure 3E; cluster coordinates and statistics in eTable 8 inSupplement 2). No group × time interactions were found for aPFC- or aIns-seeded connectivity to default mode network regions or for aMCC-seeded connectivity to S1.

    Discussion

    PRT yielded large reductions in CBP intensity relative to open-label placebo and usual care control conditions in a community sample, with nearly two-thirds of randomized patients and 73% of those initiating PRT reporting they were pain-free or nearly pain-free at posttreatment. Large effects of PRT on pain continued at 1-year follow-up. PRT also reduced experimentally evoked back pain and spontaneous pain during fMRI with large effect sizes, and several secondary outcomes (eg, disability and anger) also improved for PRT relative to the control groups.

    PRT targets primary (nociplastic) pain by shifting patients’ beliefs about the causes and threat value of pain. It presents pain as a reversible, brain-generated phenomenon not indicative of peripheral pathology, consistent with active inference and constructionist accounts of interoception and pain.18,19,22-27 PRT builds on and extends existing psychological treatment models. Cognitive-behavioral, acceptance-based, and mindfulness-based interventions typically aim to improve functioning by decreasing pain catastrophizing, enhancing pain coping or acceptance, and promoting engagement in valued life activities.41,44,46,48,62 Exposure-based treatments share with PRT an emphasis that painful activities are not injurious,42,63-65 but do not emphasize reappraising pain sensations and reattributing the causes of pain. Some pain neuroscience education interventions present pain in a similar way as PRT,43 though they typically lack guided exposure and reappraisal exercises.

    Large reductions in pain are rarely observed in CBP psychological treatment trials.11,12 Relatively unique components of PRT potentially contributing to the observed effects include (1) an in-depth medical and psychological assessment generating personalized evidence for centralized pain; (2) reattribution of pain to reversible learning- and affect-related brain processes rather than bodily injury; and (3) a unique combination of cognitive, somatic, and exposure-based techniques supporting pain reappraisal (eDiscussion inSupplement 2).

    Correlational and mediational analysis results support changes in fear-inducing pain beliefs as a potential PRT mechanism. Effects of PRT on pain beliefs were also mediated by pain intensity reductions, perhaps because pain reductions promote beliefs in pain modifiability (eDiscussion inSupplement 2). Changes in pain beliefs are not unique to PRT, but PRT may more strongly change these beliefs compared with existing therapies (eTable 6 inSupplement 2).

    These hypothesized mechanisms are consistent with extinction-based treatment approaches to anxiety disorders.42,65 For example, 85% of patients became free of panic symptoms following treatment focused on reappraising somatic symptoms as caused by nondangerous central nervous system processes (eg, false alarms).66

    PRT reduced evoked pain-related activity in aPFC, aMCC, and aIns. The aPFC and adjacent dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) are implicated in the detection and inhibition of pain.67 aPFC reductions following PRT suggest a potential reduction of pain-related signals or decreased prioritization of pain control. The aMCC and aIns are cortical convergence zones in the construction of negative affect in pain and other domains.20,68-70 Cognitive pain regulation strategies, including mindful acceptance38,39 and placebo analgesia,24,25,28 have been found to reduce aMCC and aIns responses to pain, demonstrating parallels between experimental findings and our clinical findings. The aIns reductions in our study were not specific to PRT vs placebo and may reflect processes common to both these interventions.

    PRT also increased aPFC and aIns connectivity to S1, aligning with previous findings that cognitive behavioral therapy for fibromyalgia57 and acupuncture for CBP55 increased aIns-S1 connectivity. Increased aPFC and aIns connectivity to S1 may reflect increased attention to somatosensory input in constructing pain.71 This is congruent with mindfulness-based treatments promoting nonreactive attention to bodily sensations, reducing catastrophizing.38,39,48,71 Yet, increased S1 connectivity has also been associated with increased clinical pain,72 and the role of S1 connectivity remains unclear.55 PRT vs usual care also increased aMCC-precuneus connectivity, with intermediate effects observed in participants receiving placebo treatment. Altered default mode connectivity has often been reported in chronic pain, although typically with heightened connectivity for patients vs controls (eDiscussion inSupplement 2).36,54,56,58,59

    Limitations

    Quiz Ref IDThis study has limitations. The study sample was relatively well educated and active and reported long-standing low to moderate pain and disability at baseline. The physician and therapists were experts in the treatment model. Future studies should test generalizability to other patient populations, therapists, and treatment contexts (eDiscussion inSupplement 2). The fMRI effect sizes were modest, with some results not surviving whole-brain correction (eMethods inSupplement 2). Future trials should test PRT efficacy relative to leading psychological and medical treatments (eDiscussion inSupplement 2).

    Conclusions

    Overall, our findings raise key possibilities about the nature and treatment of primary CBP. Changing fear- and avoidance-inducing beliefs about the causes and threat value of pain may provide substantial, durable pain relief for people with primary CBP.

    Back to top
    Article Information

    Accepted for Publication: July 27, 2021.

    Published Online: September 29, 2021. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.2669

    Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of theCC-BY License. © 2021 Ashar YK et al.JAMA Psychiatry.

    Corresponding Author: Tor D. Wager, PhD, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, 352 Moore Hall, HB 6207, Hanover, New Hampshire 03755 ([email protected]); Yoni K. Ashar, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Weill Cornell Medical College, 1300 York Ave, New York, NY 10065 ([email protected]).

    Author Contributions: Drs Ashar and Wager had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

    Concept and design: Ashar, Gordon, Schubiner, Uipi, Knight, Geuter, Dimidjian, Wager.

    Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Ashar, Gordon, Schubiner, Uipi, Knight, Anderson, Carlisle, Polisky, Geuter, Flood, Kragel, Lumley, Wager.

    Drafting of the manuscript: Ashar, Gordon, Schubiner, Polisky, Lumley, Wager.

    Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

    Statistical analysis: Ashar, Polisky, Geuter, Lumley, Wager.

    Obtained funding: Gordon, Schubiner, Flood, Wager.

    Administrative, technical, or material support: Ashar, Uipi, Knight, Anderson, Carlisle, Polisky, Kragel, Dimidjian, Lumley, Wager.

    Supervision: Ashar, Gordon, Wager.

    Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Ashar reports grants from the National Institutes of Health during the conduct of the study and personal fees from UnitedHealth Group, Lin Health, Inc, Pain Reprocessing Therapy Center, Inc, and Mental Health Partners of Boulder County outside the submitted work. Mr Gordon is a consultant with UnitedHealth Group, director of the Pain Psychology Center and the Pain Reprocessing Therapy Center, and is the author of the bookThe Way Out. Dr Schubiner is the co-owner of Freedom From Chronic Pain, Inc, earns book royalties forUnlearn Your Pain, Unlearn Your Anxiety and Depression andHidden From View; serves as a consultant with UnitedHealth Group, Karuna Labs, and Curable Health; and receives personal fees from OVID Dx outside the submitted work. Mrs Uipi serves as a consultant for UnitedHealth Group. Dr Dimidjian reports being a co-founder of Mindful Noggin, Inc, and received royalties from Guilford Press and Wolters Kluwer as well as funding from The National Institutes of Health. Dr Lumley reports personal fees from CognifiSense, Inc, outside the submitted work. Dr Wager reports grants from the National Institutes of Health and the Foundation for the Study of the Therapeutic Encounter, and funding to support trainees from the Radiological Society of North America and the German Research Foundation; he is on the Scientific Advisory Board of Curable Health. No other disclosures were reported.

    Funding and Support: This study was funded by National Institutes of Health grants R01 DA035484 (Dr Wager), R01 MH076136 (Dr Wager), National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences grant TL1-TR-002386 (Dr Ashar), Radiological Society of North America (Dr Flood), German Research Foundation grant GE 2774/1-1 (Dr Geuter), the Psychophysiologic Disorders Association, the Foundation for the Study of the Therapeutic Encounter, and community donations.

    Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

    Data Sharing Statement: SeeSupplement 3.

    Additional Information: Deidentified demographic and clinical outcomes data and subject-level functional magnetic resonance imaging statistical parameter maps for evoked pain and seed connectivity are provided here:https://figshare.com/s/1840dc4c0e236a7072ca

    References
    1.
    Dahlhamer  J, Lucas  J, Zelaya  C,  et al.  Prevalence of chronic pain and high-impact chronic pain among adults—United States, 2016.  MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2018;67(36):1001-1006. doi:10.15585/mmwr.mm6736a2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    2.
    Institute of Medicine. Relieving Pain in America: A Blueprint for Transforming Prevention, Care, Education, and Research. National Academies Press; 2011. doi:10.17226/13172
    3.
    Hartvigsen  J, Hancock  MJ, Kongsted  A,  et al; Lancet Low Back Pain Series Working Group.  What low back pain is and why we need to pay attention.  Lancet. 2018;391(10137):2356-2367. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30480-XPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    4.
    Maher  C, Underwood  M, Buchbinder  R.  Non-specific low back pain.  Lancet. 2017;389(10070):736-747. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30970-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    5.
    Vlaeyen  JWS, Maher  CG, Wiech  K,  et al  Low back pain.  Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2018;4(1):52. doi:10.1038/s41572-018-0052-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    6.
    Henschke  N, Maher  CG, Refshauge  KM,  et al.  Prevalence of and screening for serious spinal pathology in patients presenting to primary care settings with acute low back pain.  Arthritis Rheum. 2009;60(10):3072-3080. doi:10.1002/art.24853PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    7.
    Deyo  RA, Weinstein  JN.  Low back pain.  N Engl J Med. 2001;344(5):363-370. doi:10.1056/NEJM200102013440508PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    8.
    Karshikoff  B, Jensen  KB, Kosek  E,  et al  Why sickness hurts: a central mechanism for pain induced by peripheral inflammation.  Brain Behav Immun. 2016;57:38-46. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2016.04.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    9.
    Nicholas  M, Vlaeyen  JWS, Rief  W,  et al; IASP Taskforce for the Classification of Chronic Pain.  The IASP classification of chronic pain for ICD-11: chronic primary pain.  Pain. 2019;160(1):28-37. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001390PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    10.
    Clauw  DJ.  Diagnosing and treating chronic musculoskeletal pain based on the underlying mechanism(s).  Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol. 2015;29(1):6-19. doi:10.1016/j.berh.2015.04.024PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    11.
    Williams  ACC, Fisher  E, Hearn  L, Eccleston  C.  Psychological therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults.  Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;8(8):CD007407. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD007407.pub4PubMedGoogle Scholar
    12.
    Chou  R, Deyo  R, Friedly  J,  et al.  Nonpharmacologic therapies for low back pain: a systematic review for an American College of physicians clinical practice guideline.  Ann Intern Med. 2017;166(7):493-505. doi:10.7326/M16-2459PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    13.
    Woolf  CJ.  Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain.  Pain. 2011;152(3)(suppl):S2-S15. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    14.
    Kuner  R, Flor  H.  Structural plasticity and reorganisation in chronic pain.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;18(1):20-30. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.162PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    15.
    Corder  G, Ahanonu  B, Grewe  BF, Wang  D, Schnitzer  MJ, Scherrer  G.  An amygdalar neural ensemble that encodes the unpleasantness of pain.  Science. 2019;363(6424):276-281. doi:10.1126/science.aap8586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    16.
    Dale  J, Zhou  H, Zhang  Q,  et al.  Scaling up cortical control inhibits pain.  Cell Rep. 2018;23(5):1301-1313. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2018.03.139PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    17.
    Hua  T, Chen  B, Lu  D,  et al.  General anesthetics activate a potent central pain-suppression circuit in the amygdala.  Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(7):854-868. doi:10.1038/s41593-020-0632-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    18.
    Kleckner  IR, Zhang  J, Touroutoglou  A,  et al.  Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans.  Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1:0069. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0069PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    19.
    Barrett  LF.  The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(1):1-23. doi:10.1093/scan/nsx060PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    20.
    Barrett  LF, Simmons  WK.  Interoceptive predictions in the brain.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(7):419-429. doi:10.1038/nrn3950PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    21.
    Sterling  P.  Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation.  Physiol Behav. 2012;106(1):5-15. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    22.
    Baliki  MN, Apkarian  AV.  Nociception, pain, negative moods, and behavior selection.  Neuron. 2015;87(3):474-491. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    23.
    Petzschner  FH, Weber  LAE, Gard  T, Stephan  KE.  Computational psychosomatics and computational psychiatry: toward a joint framework for differential diagnosis.  Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82(6):421-430. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.05.012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    24.
    Büchel  C, Geuter  S, Sprenger  C, Eippert  F.  Placebo analgesia: a predictive coding perspective.  Neuron. 2014;81(6):1223-1239. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    25.
    Tracey  I.  Getting the pain you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans.  Nat Med. 2010;16(11):1277-1283. doi:10.1038/nm.2229PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    26.
    Kaptchuk  TJ, Hemond  CC, Miller  FG.  Placebos in chronic pain: evidence, theory, ethics, and use in clinical practice.  BMJ. 2020;370:m1668. doi:10.1136/bmj.m1668PubMedGoogle Scholar
    27.
    Seymour  B.  Pain: a precision signal for reinforcement learning and control.  Neuron. 2019;101(6):1029-1041. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.055PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    28.
    Ashar  YK, Chang  LJ, Wager  TD.  Brain mechanisms of the placebo effect: an affective appraisal account.  Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13(1):73-98. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    29.
    Jepma  M, Koban  L, van Doorn  J, Jones  M, Wager  TD.  Behavioural and neural evidence for self-reinforcing expectancy effects on pain.  Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(11):838-855. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0455-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    30.
    Vlaeyen  JWS, Crombez  G.  Behavioral conceptualization and treatment of chronic pain.  Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2020;16:187-212. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095744PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    31.
    Meulders  A.  Fear in the context of pain: Lessons learned from 100 years of fear conditioning research.  Behav Res Ther. 2020;131:103635. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2020.103635PubMedGoogle Scholar
    32.
    Hashmi  JA, Baliki  MN, Huang  L,  et al.  Shape shifting pain: chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits.  Brain. 2013;136(Pt 9):2751-2768. doi:10.1093/brain/awt211PubMedGoogle Scholar
    33.
    Napadow  V, Harris  RE.  What has functional connectivity and chemical neuroimaging in fibromyalgia taught us about the mechanisms and management of ‘centralized’ pain?  Arthritis Res Ther. 2014;16(5):425. doi:10.1186/s13075-014-0425-0PubMedGoogle Scholar
    34.
    Roy  M, Shohamy  D, Daw  N, Jepma  M, Wimmer  GE, Wager  TD.  Representation of aversive prediction errors in the human periaqueductal gray.  Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(11):1607-1612. doi:10.1038/nn.3832PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    35.
    Čeko  M, Shir  Y, Ouellet  JA, Ware  MA, Stone  LS, Seminowicz  DA.  Partial recovery of abnormal insula and dorsolateral prefrontal connectivity to cognitive networks in chronic low back pain after treatment.  Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(6):2075-2092. doi:10.1002/hbm.22757PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    36.
    Martucci  KT, Shirer  WR, Bagarinao  E,  et al.  The posterior medial cortex in urologic chronic pelvic pain syndrome: detachment from default mode network—a resting-state study from the MAPP Research Network.  Pain. 2015;156(9):1755-1764. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000238PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    37.
    Woo  C-W, Roy  M, Buhle  JT, Wager  TD.  Distinct brain systems mediate the effects of nociceptive input and self-regulation on pain.  PLoS Biol. 2015;13(1):e1002036. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002036PubMedGoogle Scholar
    38.
    Kober  H, Buhle  J, Weber  J, Ochsner  KN, Wager  TD.  Let it be: mindful acceptance down-regulates pain and negative emotion.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(11):1147-1158. doi:10.1093/scan/nsz104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    39.
    Lutz  A, McFarlin  DR, Perlman  DM, Salomons  TV, Davidson  RJ.  Altered anterior insula activation during anticipation and experience of painful stimuli in expert meditators.  Neuroimage. 2013;64(1):538-546. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.030PubMedGoogle Scholar
    40.
    Xiao  L, Yank  V, Ma  J.  Algorithm for balancing both continuous and categorical covariates in randomized controlled trials.  Comput Methods Programs Biomed. 2012;108(3):1185-1190. doi:10.1016/j.cmpb.2012.06.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    41.
    Thorn  BE, Eyer  JC, Van Dyke  BP,  et al.  Literacy-adapted cognitive behavioral therapy versus education for chronic pain at low-income clinics: a randomized controlled trial.  Ann Intern Med. 2018;168(7):471-480. doi:10.7326/M17-0972PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    42.
    Vlaeyen  JWS, Morley  S, Linton  S, Boersma  K, De Jong  J. Pain-Related Fear: Exposure-Based Treatment for Chronic Pain. IASP Press; 2012.
    43.
    Moseley  GL, Butler  DS.  Fifteen years of explaining pain: the past, present, and future.  J Pain. 2015;16(9):807-813. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    44.
    McCracken  LM, Vowles  KE.  Acceptance and commitment therapy and mindfulness for chronic pain: model, process, and progress.  Am Psychol. 2014;69(2):178-187. doi:10.1037/a0035623PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    45.
    Lumley  MA, Schubiner  H, Lockhart  NA,  et al.  Emotional awareness and expression therapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, and education for fibromyalgia: a cluster-randomized controlled trial.  Pain. 2017;158(12):2354-2363. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001036PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    46.
    Cherkin  DC, Sherman  KJ, Balderson  BH,  et al.  Effect of mindfulness-based stress reduction vs cognitive behavioral therapy or usual care on back pain and functional limitations in adults with chronic low back pain: a randomized clinical trial.  JAMA. 2016;315(12):1240-1249. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.2323PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    47.
    Kabat-Zinn  J, Lipworth  L, Burney  R.  The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain.  J Behav Med. 1985;8(2):163-190. doi:10.1007/BF00845519PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    48.
    Garland  EL, Manusov  EG, Froeliger  B, Kelly  A, Williams  JM, Howard  MO.  Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for chronic pain and prescription opioid misuse: results from an early-stage randomized controlled trial.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 2014;82(3):448-459. doi:10.1037/a0035798PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    49.
    Kaptchuk  TJ, Friedlander  E, Kelley  JM,  et al.  Placebos without deception: a randomized controlled trial in irritable bowel syndrome.  PLoS One. 2010;5(12):e15591. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0015591PubMedGoogle Scholar
    50.
    Locher  C, Frey Nascimento  A, Kirsch  I, Kossowsky  J, Meyer  A, Gaab  J.  Is the rationale more important than deception? a randomized controlled trial of open-label placebo analgesia.  Pain. 2017;158(12):2320-2328. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    51.
    Kleine-Borgmann  J, Schmidt  K, Hellmann  A, Bingel  U.  Effects of open-label placebo on pain, functional disability, and spine mobility in patients with chronic back pain: a randomized controlled trial.  Pain. 2019;160(12):2891-2897. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001683PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    52.
    Lembo  A, Kelley  JM, Nee  J,  et al.  Open-label placebo vs double-blind placebo for irritable bowel syndrome: a randomized clinical trial.  Pain. 2021. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002234PubMedGoogle Scholar
    53.
    Schubiner  H, Betzold  M. Unlearn Your Pain. Mind Body Publishing; 2010.
    54.
    Napadow  V, Kim  J, Clauw  DJ, Harris  RE.  Decreased intrinsic brain connectivity is associated with reduced clinical pain in fibromyalgia.  Arthritis Rheum. 2012;64(7):2398-2403. doi:10.1002/art.34412PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    55.
    Mawla  I, Ichesco  E, Zöllner  HJ,  et al.  Greater Somatosensory Afference With Acupuncture Increases Primary Somatosensory Connectivity and Alleviates Fibromyalgia Pain via Insular γ-Aminobutyric Acid: A Randomized Neuroimaging Trial.  Arthritis Rheumatol. 2021;73(7):1318-1328. doi:10.1002/art.41620PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    56.
    Bushnell  MC, Ceko  M, Low  LA.  Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(7):502-511. doi:10.1038/nrn3516PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    57.
    Lazaridou  A, Kim  J, Cahalan  CM,  et al.  Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) on brain connectivity supporting catastrophizing in fibromyalgia.  Clin J Pain. 2017;33(3):215-221. doi:10.1097/AJP.0000000000000422PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    58.
    Kim  J, Mawla  I, Kong  J,  et al.  Somatotopically specific primary somatosensory connectivity to salience and default mode networks encodes clinical pain.  Pain. 2019;160(7):1594-1605. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001541PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    59.
    Kong  J, Spaeth  RB, Wey  H-Y,  et al.  S1 is associated with chronic low back pain: a functional and structural MRI study.  Mol Pain. 2013;9(1):43. doi:10.1186/1744-8069-9-43PubMedGoogle Scholar
    60.
    Adler  NE, Epel  ES, Castellazzo  G, Ickovics  JR.  Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women.  Health Psychol. 2000;19(6):586-592. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    61.
    Brinjikji  W, Luetmer  PH, Comstock  B,  et al.  Systematic literature review of imaging features of spinal degeneration in asymptomatic populations.  AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2015;36(4):811-816. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A4173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    62.
    Keefe  FJ, Caldwell  DS, Williams  DA,  et al  Pain coping skills training in the management of osteoarthritic knee pain: a comparative study.  Behav Ther. 1990;21(1):49-62. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80188-1Google ScholarCrossref
    63.
    Logan  DE, Carpino  EA, Chiang  G,  et al.  A day-hospital approach to treatment of pediatric complex regional pain syndrome: initial functional outcomes.  Clin J Pain. 2012;28(9):766-774. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182457619PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    64.
    Sherry  DD, Wallace  CA, Kelley  C, Kidder  M, Sapp  L.  Short- and long-term outcomes of children with complex regional pain syndrome type I treated with exercise therapy.  Clin J Pain. 1999;15(3):218-223. doi:10.1097/00002508-199909000-00009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    65.
    Craske  MG, Kircanski  K, Zelikowsky  M, Mystkowski  J, Chowdhury  N, Baker  A.  Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy.  Behav Res Ther. 2008;46(1):5-27. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    66.
    Barlow  DH, Craske  MG, Cerny  JA, Klosko  JS.  Behavioral treatment of panic disorder.  Behav Ther. 1989;20(2):261-282. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80073-5Google ScholarCrossref
    67.
    Seminowicz  DA, Moayedi  M.  The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in acute and chronic pain.  J Pain. 2017;18(9):1027-1035. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2017.03.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    68.
    Craig  AD.  How do you feel—now? the anterior insula and human awareness.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):59-70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    69.
    Lindquist  KA, Wager  TD, Kober  H, Bliss-Moreau  E, Barrett  LF.  The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review.  Behav Brain Sci. 2012;35(3):121-143. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000446PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    70.
    Geuter  S, Boll  S, Eippert  F, Büchel  C.  Functional dissociation of stimulus intensity encoding and predictive coding of pain in the insula.  Elife. 2017;6:e24770. doi:10.7554/eLife.24770PubMedGoogle Scholar
    71.
    Garland  EL, Gaylord  SA, Palsson  O, Faurot  K, Douglas Mann  J, Whitehead  WE.  Therapeutic mechanisms of a mindfulness-based treatment for IBS: effects on visceral sensitivity, catastrophizing, and affective processing of pain sensations.  J Behav Med. 2012;35(6):591-602. doi:10.1007/s10865-011-9391-zPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    72.
    Kim  J, Loggia  ML, Cahalan  CM,  et al.  The somatosensory link in fibromyalgia: functional connectivity of the primary somatosensory cortex is altered by sustained pain and is associated with clinical/autonomic dysfunction.  Arthritis Rheumatol. 2015;67(5):1395-1405. doi:10.1002/art.39043PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
    X
    .
    ×
    Access your subscriptions

    Add or change institution
    Free access to newly published articles

    To register for email alerts, access free PDF, and more
    Purchase access

    Get full journal access for 1 year
    Get unlimited access and a printable PDF ($40.00)—
    Sign in or create a free account
    Rent this article from DeepDyve
    Access your subscriptions

    Add or change institution
    Free access to newly published articles

    To register for email alerts, access free PDF, and more
    Purchase access

    Get full journal access for 1 year
    Get unlimited access and a printable PDF ($40.00)—
    Sign in or create a free account
    Rent this article from DeepDyve
    Sign in to access free PDF

    Add or change institution
    Free access to newly published articles

    To register for email alerts, access free PDF, and more
    Save your search

    Free access to newly published articles

    To register for email alerts, access free PDF, and more
    Purchase access

    Customize your interests

    Free access to newly published articles

    To register for email alerts, access free PDF, and more
    Create a personal account or sign in to:
    • Register for email alerts with links to free full-text articles
    • Access PDFs of free articles
    • Manage your interests
    • Save searches and receive search alerts
      Privacy Policy
      Make a comment

      Free access to newly published articles

      To register for email alerts, access free PDF, and more
      Create a personal account or sign in to:
      • Register for email alerts with links to free full-text articles
      • Access PDFs of free articles
      • Manage your interests
      • Save searches and receive search alerts
        Privacy Policy

        [8]ページ先頭

        ©2009-2025 Movatter.jp