Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Jump to content
WikipediaThe Free Encyclopedia
Search

Unami language

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Language spoken by the Lenape people
Unami
Wënami èlixsuwakàn
Native toEastern United States
RegionAround the lowerDelaware andHudson rivers in theUnited States; some Unami groups inOklahoma
EthnicityLenape
ExtinctAugust 31, 2002, with the death of Edward Thompson[1]
RevivalTaught as a second language
Language codes
ISO 639-3unm
Glottologunam1242
ELPUnami
Map showing the aboriginal boundaries of Delaware territories, with Munsee territory and Unami dialectal divisions indicated. The territory of the Unalachtigo dialect of Unami is not clearly indicated, but is presumed to be approximately in the area of "Sankhikan" on the map.
This article containsIPA phonetic symbols. Without properrendering support, you may seequestion marks, boxes, or other symbols instead ofUnicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, seeHelp:IPA.
PersonWënami,
Lënape
PeopleWënamiyok,
Lënapeyok
LanguageWënami èlixsuwakàn,
Lënapei èlixsuwakàn
CountryWënamihòkink,
Lënapehòkink

Unami (Delaware:Wënami èlixsuwakàn)[4] is anAlgonquian language initially spoken by theLenape people in the late 17th century and the early 18th century, in the southern two-thirds of present-dayNew Jersey, southeasternPennsylvania, and the northern two-thirds ofDelaware. The Lenape later migrated, largely settling inOntario, Canada andOklahoma. Today, it is only spoken as a second language.

Unami is one of twoDelaware languages; the other isMunsee. The last fluent Unami speaker in the United States, Edward Thompson, of thefederally recognizedDelaware Tribe of Indians, died on August 31, 2002.[1] His sisterNora Thompson Dean (1907–1984) provided valuable information about the language to linguists and other scholars.

Lenni-Lenape literally means 'Men of Men', but is translated to mean 'Original People'. The Lenape names for the areas they inhabited wereScheyichbi (i.e. New Jersey), which means 'water's edge', andLenapehoking, meaning 'in the land of the Delaware Indians'. It describes the ancient homeland of all Delaware Indians, both Unami and Munsee.[5][6] The English named the river running through much of the traditional range of the Lenape after the first governor of theJamestown Colony,Lord De La Warr, and consequently referred to the people who lived around the river as "Delaware Indians".

History

[edit]

Unami is anEastern Algonquian language. The hypothetical common ancestor language from which the Eastern Algonquian languages descend is Proto-Eastern Algonquian (PEA). An intermediate group,Delawarean, that is a descendant of Proto-Eastern Algonquian consists of Mahican and Common Delaware, the latter being a further subgroup comprising Munsee Delaware and Unami Delaware.[7] The justification for Delawarean as an intermediate subgroup rests upon the high degree of similarity between Mahican and the two Delaware languages, but relatively little detailed argumentation in support of Delawarean has been adduced.[7][8][9]

Compared to Munsee, Unami has undergone extensive phonological innovation, coupled with morphologicalregularization.[10]

The PEA vowel system consisted of four long vowels*i·, *o·, *e·, *a·, and two short vowels*a andə. The vowel history is as follows:*i· (from PEA merger of Proto-Algonquian (PA*i· and*i to PEA*i·),*o· (from PEA merger of PA*o· and*o ),*e· (from Proto-Algonquian*e·), and*a· (from Proto-Algonquian*a·; the short vowels are (from Proto-Algonquian*e), and*a (from Proto-Algonquian*a). This system was continued down to Common Delaware,[11] but Munsee and Unami have innovated separately with respect to the vowel systems.

Revival attempts

[edit]

The dominant modern version of the Southern Unami dialect calledLenape is taught by theDelaware Tribe of Indians, headquartered inBartlesville, Oklahoma, which manages the Lenape Language Preservation Project.[12] The same dialect was spoken by theDelaware Nation inAnadarko in the southwestern part of Oklahoma. Both Oklahoma and Delaware tribes have recorded native speakers and produced written lessons for instruction, which are available for sale from Various Indian Peoples Publishing Company, located inTexas. These efforts, in conjunction with other community efforts comprise an attempt to preserve the language.

Some descriptions of the Northern Unami dialect as spoken during the 18th century are given by Moravian missionaryJohn Heckewelder.[13]

Phonology

[edit]

Consonants

[edit]

Unami has been analyzed as having contrastivegeminate and non-geminateobstruent consonants, although this contrast is relatively weak.[14] A full analysis of the status of the geminates, also known as long consonants, is not available, and more than one analysis of Delaware consonants has been proposed. The long consonants are described as having low functional yield, and they differentiate relatively few pairs of words but occur in contrasting environments.[15] Some examples of contrastive geminate pairs include:k·ə́ntka·n 'then you (sg.) danced' versuskə́ntka·n 'then there was dancing';p·ɔ́·m 'his thigh' versuspɔ́·m 'the ham'; andnsa·s·a·k·ənə́mən 'I stuck it out repeatedly' versusnsa·sa·k·ənə́mən 'I stuck it out slowly'.

There are also rules that lengthen consonants in certain environments.[16] The length mark(ː) is used to indicate gemination of a preceding consonant orvowel length, although in the literature on Unami the raised dot (·) is often used for these purposes, as other diacritics may be used above vowels (see below).[17]

In the following chart, the usual transcription used in the sources is given with the IPA in brackets.

Consonants
BilabialDentalPostalveolarVelarGlottal
Plosivep[p]t[t̪]č[tʃ]k[k]
Fricatives[s]š[ʃ]x[x]h[h]
Nasalm[m]n[n]
Laterall[l]
Semivowelw[w]j[j]

Vowels

[edit]

Unami vowels are presented as organized into contrasting long–short pairs.[18] One asymmetry is that high short/u/ is paired with long/oː/, and the pairing of long and short/ə/ is noteworthy./ə/ and/o/ are not distinguishable before/w/,/m/, and/kw/. Additionally, vowels are classified as strong and weak, which plays an important role in determining stress (see below). Long vowels and vowels before consonant clusters are automatically strong. Certain short vowels, which are differentiated with abreve/ĭ,ĕ,ŏ/ – are also strong vowels because they are treated morphophonemically as long vowels, even though they are pronounced as short. In a sequence of syllables containing a short vowel followed by a consonant (C) or consonant and/w/ (Cw), the odd-numbered vowels are weak, and the even numbered vowels are strong.[vague]

Furthermore, some short vowels are strong even in a weakening environment; such exceptions are often marked with agrave accent. Additionally, some vowels which are unaffected by predicted vowel syncope are marked with anacute accent.[19] There is a predictable tendency, additionally, to nasalize and lengthen a vowel before/ns/ and/nš/, so that/lowé·nso/ ('his name is [such]') is realized closer to[luwé̹·su] from underlying/ələwe·nsəw/.[20]

Long vowels
FrontCentralBack
Close
Midəːɔː
Open
Short vowels
FrontCentralBack
Closeiu
Mideəɔ
Opena

Syllable structure

[edit]

Syllable structure is diverse, permitting a certain amount of consonant clustering. The following consonant clusters can occur:[21]

  • /m,n/ (which are realized ashomorganic nasals) +/p,t,k,s,tʃ,ʃ/
  • /h/ +/p,t,tʃ,k,m,l/
  • /s/ +/p,k/
  • /x/ +/p,k/
  • /tʃ/ +/k/

Additionally, certain consonants may combine with the semivowel/w/. Some underlying forms may also contain/sw/ and/šw/, but these are always removed by morphophonemic processes.[22]

  • /p,m,k,h/ +/w/

Stress

[edit]

Stress is generally predictable in Unami. The rightmost nonfinal strong vowel is stressed, or a strong vowel in final position if it is the only one in the word. Often when stress would be expected to fall on theantepenult it is shifted to thepenult. This change is found in three conjunct endings:/-ak/,/-at/, and/-an/. In the last case, the accent shifts to the penultimate/-an/ only if it would otherwise fall on an antepenultimate short vowel, and if the consonant between them is voiced.[23]

Phonological processes

[edit]

Unami phonology is extremely complex, with variousmorphophonological rules, and a theoretical form usually undergoes a set of predictable phonological processes to produce the true form found in speech. There are about 17 such rules common to bothMunsee and Unami, and another 28 unique to Unami, though this analysis ignores predictable exceptions, such as the class of static words which may skip many of these rules. These rules govern things such as consonant lengthening/shortening, vowel syncopation, metathesis, vowel coloring, etc.[24]

A list of processes unique to Unami follow.[25] These are written in linguistic notation. Thus,{ə,a} → ∅ / _{h, x}V when {ə,a} are weak means that the sounds/ə/ and/a/ become null (disappear) in the context of when they are weak and appear before either/h/ or/x/ and another vowel. The slash means 'in the context of', and the underscore _ indicates where the/ə/ or/a/ must occur. In some notations the pound symbol (#) appears, indicating word boundaries (either the beginning or end). Regular parenthesis indicate optional conditions when framing phonemes or additional information about phonemes: "C=stop". The capital letters C, V, and N mean 'consonant', 'vowel', and 'nasal' respectively.

  • U-1: Weak and strong vowel marking
  • U-2: Weak short vowel loss before gutturals/h/ and/x/:/kənalhó·xwe/ ('you walk upstream') versus/nalahó·xwe·(w)/ ('he walks upstream')
    • {ə,a}when/ə,a/ are weak→ ∅ / _{h, x}V
  • U-3: Vowel-coloring; underlying/ə/ may be color to/i//o/ or/a/ in various environments.
    • ə → o / _ {(k)w} -- although Goddard notes that this orthography may be imperfect because surface-phonemic/ə/ and/o/ are not distinct before/w/,/m/ and/kw/. Cf./pko/ ('vegetable gum', underlying/pəkəw/) with/mpok·ó·yom/ ('my gum') but/mpək·ó·he/ ('I gather gum').
    • ə → i / _ {y}
    • ə → o / _ h{p, kw, m, w}
    • ə → i / _ helsewhere
    • ə → o / _ {Np, Nkw} and // w_Nk
    • ə → i / _ Nkelsewhere
    • ə → o / _x{p, kw}
    • ə → a / _xelsewhere
  • U-4: Vowel shortening before primary cluster of a nasal and another consonant
    • V̅ → V̌ / _ NC
  • U-5: Semivowel assimilation
    • {w, y}h → hh / V̌_
  • U-6: Stop lengthening
    • C^(vl≠h) → C· / V_(s, š, x)where V is strong
  • U-7: /h/-metathesis
    • V̌hC → hV̌Cexcept / VC_ where V is weak
  • U-8: An adjustment in vowel length before/hC/ (an/h/ and another consonant)
    • V → V̌ / _hC(voiced C)
    • V → V̅ / _hC(voiceless C)
  • U-9:/h/-loss before stop consonant
    • hC(C=stop) → C
  • U-10: Nasal assimilation, part 1
    • NC(C=stop) → homorganic nasal + voiced stop
    • NC(C=continuant)nC / V_
  • U-11: Vowel-weakening andsyncope (with certain exceptions)
    • a → ah / CV(/a/ is weak; C=voiceless) except some a(a=weak) → ∅ / #n_CV(C=voiceless)
    • ə → ∅ / _CV(/ə/ is weak; C=voiceless)and / l_{n, l}, y_l, w_w, m_mand / #(n)_{n, l}V
  • U-12: Nasal assimilation, part 2
    • nC(C=voiceless; C≠/x,h/) → homorganic nasal + C(C=voiced) / #_
    • VnC → Ṽ·C(most speakers)
  • U-13: Voiced consonant assimilation
    • C(x)C(y)(C=voiced constituant) → CyCy
  • U-14: Vowel syncope before/xCV/, in which exceptions are marked with a grave accent
    • V̌ → ∅ / _{x, s, š}CVexcept / VC_(where V=weak)
  • U-15:/h/-loss in clusters
    • h → ∅ / _{CC, C#, Cah}
  • U-16: Vowel shortening
    • V̅ → V̌ / _h(ə)CVand // _CC(C=voiced) and / _C·ah
  • U-17:/mə/-loss
    • V́hməna· → V́hna·
  • U-18:/ə/-insertion
    • ∅ → ə / h_{l, n, m}and / #{l, m}_C (C≠h)
  • U-19:Metathesis of/w/. Does not affect the roots/wəl-/ 'well' and/wət-/ 'pull'.
    • w(ə)C1(w) → Cw / #_Vif C1=/p,m,k,h/
    • w(ə)C → Cw / #_{ah, a, a·}
    • {/nəw/,/kəw/} → {nw, kw} / #_{ah, a, a·}
    • w{h, x} → {hw, xw} / C_
    • x → xw / o(·)_
    • (k)wx(k) → (k)x(k)w / #_
  • U-20:/w/-coloring of/a/-vowels and/w/-loss
    • w{ah, a, a·} → {oh, ɔ, ɔ·} / {#,C,V}_where C is not/w/ and V is not/o(·)/
    • w → ∅ / V1_V2where V1 is not/o(·)/ and V2 is not/ə/
    • w → ∅ / (C)_o(·)
  • U-21:/y/-adjustment
    • y → ∅ / V̄_V1where V1 is not/ə/
    • ∅ → y / V1_V2where V1 is a front vowel and V2 is a back vowel
    • w → y / _k(only in the suffixes1/-əkw/ and1/-əke·/)
  • U-22: Final/l/-loss. The dropping of the/l/ is optional, but the option is exploited differently by the two morphemes it affects.
    • l → (l) / _#
  • U-23: Final-vowel shortening
    • V̄ → V̆ / _#
  • U-24: Final/h/-,/w/-, and/y/-loss
    • h → ∅ / _#
    • w → (w) / V̄_#
    • w → ∅ / o(·)_#
    • w → ∅ / #_tə{l, n}
    • y → ∅ / i_#
  • U-25: Initial cluster and syllable loss. Initial clusters arising from morphophonemic rules U-11 and U-14 above are simplified; many initial weak-vowel syllables are lost. There are many exceptions, however, such that Goddard does not attempt to describe the pattern.
  • U-26: Consonant-shortening
    • C·(w) → C(w) / _{ah, oh, C, #}
  • U-27: Consonant-lengthening
    • C(C=stop) → C· / #(C)hV̆_V(C)#
  • U-28: Negative vowel assimilation in forms with5/-(o·)w(i·)/, which was a recent innovation at the time Goddard was writing.
    • x(w)i → V̄xx / _#

Morphology

[edit]

Nouns

[edit]

Third person participants are marked for gender (animate versus inanimate), obviation (proximate versusobviative), and presence (nonabsentative versus absentative). Generally, the inanimate, obviative, and absentative categories are more marked than their opposites (i.e. animate, proximate, and nonabsentative), but it is not clear whether animacy or inanimacy is the more marked of the opposition. The first and second persons are not marked for presence or obviation and are always animate.[26]

Obviation

[edit]

The first mentioned and/or primary animate third person is proximate; all other third persons areobviative, unless they act in conjunction with the proximate participant. Verbs are also inflected to indicate whether the verbal action is proximate on obviate or obviate on proximate.

Presence

[edit]

Third-person participants can be marked by a special set of endings indicating their absence from the general area of the focus of discourse. For example, absentative endings are used when speaking of the deceased (even if the corpse is physically present), as in the sentenceno·lăčahko·ná·na nkahe·səná·na ('our (excl.) mother (abv.) treated us well'), in which both verb and noun are marked with the absentative/-a/ ending.

Gender

[edit]

Nouns in Unami areclassified as animate or inanimate, which is reflected in verbal conjugation. Animate nouns denote human beings, animals, spirits, trees, and certain fruits, tubers, root vegetables, and other unpredictable exceptions likeko·n ('snow') andnhíkaš ('my fingernail'). (However, berries, nuts, and vegetables growing above ground are generally inanimate.) Thus,té·hi·m ('strawberry'),xáskwi·m ('corn'),ke·skúnthak ('pumpkin'),mpi ('water'), andnhíka·t ('my leg') are inanimate, whilelə́nu ('man'),xho·k ('snake'),mahtán'tu ('Devil') andhɔ́pəni·s ('potato') are animate. However, traditionally inanimate nouns which are directly addressed or personified are treated as animate. Thus, traditionally inanimateăsǝ́n ('stone') is treated as animate in the sentencešá·i a· ăsǝ́nak kǝnčí·mowak ('the stones would immediately cry out').[27]

Verbs

[edit]

Unami is a highlyagglutinative,polysynthetic language. Verbs in Unami are marked for person and number, and contain inflectional elements of order (independent, conjunct, and imperative), aspect, and the negative.[28]

A table of the personal pronouns is given below. The first person plural ("we") may be eitherinclusive (including the addressee) or exclusive.

Personal pronouns[29]
PersonSingularPlural
Firstni·ni·ló·na (exclusive)ki·ló·na (inclusive)
Secondki·ki·ló·wa
Thirdné·k·ane·k·a·ɔ
Indefinite∅ (zero ending)∅ (zero ending)

Following are tables exemplifying verbal paradigms in Unami in the independent order, indicative mood and present tense.[30]

Animate intransitive (AI) verbs

[edit]
Animate intransitive independent indicative
Person/kəntəka·-/
'dance'
/wəm-/
'come from'
1,2nkə́ntka
/n-,k--/
no·m
/n-,k--/
3kə́ntke·(w)
/-w/
wəm
/-w/
1p,12nkəntkáhəna
/n-,k--hməna·/
no·mhóməna
/n-,k--hməna·/
2pkkəntkáhəmɔ
/k--hmwa·/
ko·mhómɔ
/k--hmwa·/
3pkəntké·yɔk
/-wak/
mo·k
/-wak/
Xkə́ntkan
/-n/
xahé·lən
/-n/

Transitive animate (TA) verbs

[edit]
Transitive animate independent indicative, theme 1
Objectivemi·l-
'give to'
Absolutelo·sw
'burn'
Person33p-
1,2nǝmí·la
/n-,k--a·/
nǝmi·lá·ɔk
/n-,k--a·wak/
nló·sa
/n-,k--a·/
3mwi·lá·ɔ
/w--a·wa(l)/
mwi·lá·ɔ
/w--a·wa(l)/
ló·se·(w)
/-e·w/
1p,12nǝmi·lá·wǝna
/n-,k--a·wǝna·/
nǝmi·la·wǝná·na(·)k
/n-,k--a·wǝnă̆·k/
nlo·sáhǝna
/n-,k--a·hmǝna·/
2pkǝmi·láwwa
/k--a·wǝwa·/
kǝmi·láwwa·ɔk
/k--a·wǝwa·wak/
kǝlo·sáhǝmɔ
/k--a·hmwa·/
3pmwi·lawwá·ɔ
/w--a·wǝwa·wa(l)/
mwi·lawwá·ɔ
/w--a·wǝwa·wa(l)/
lo·sé·yɔk
/-ǝ·wak/
Xmí·la·(w)
/a·w/
mí·l·á·ɔk
/a·wak/
N/A

Inanimate intransitive (II) verbs

[edit]
Inanimate intransitive independent indicative
Personmàxhe·-
'be red'
alǝt-
'rot'
INmáxke·(w)
/-w/
alǝ́t
/-w/
IN pl.maxké·yɔ(l)
/-wa(l)/
alǝ́t·o(·l)
/-wa(l)/

Transitive inanimate (TI) verbs

[edit]

The TI themes have the same inflection as AI stems for all conjuncts. (Indefinite subject forms of consonant-final themes are not attested, but the vowel-final themes follow the AI pattern.) Three forms are illustrated from each type.

Transitive inanimate independent indicative
TypeSubjects
233pl
1apǝnáman
/-aman/
pǝnánk
/-ank/
pǝnamhíti·t
/-amǝh(ǝ)ti·t/
1bló·sǝman
/-ǝman/
ló·sink
/-ǝnk/
lo·sǝmíhti·t
/-ǝmǝh(ǝ)ti·t/
2wǝlí·taɔn
/-awan/
wǝlí·ta·kw
/-a·kw/
wǝli·tóhti·t
/-o·h(ǝ)ti·t/
3(/-C/)né·man
/-an/
nenk
/-ǝk/
ne·mhíti·t
/-ǝ·h(ǝ)ti·t/
3(/-V)mí·č·iyan
/-yan/
mí·č·i·t
/-t/
mi·č·íhti·t
/-h(ǝ)ti·t/

Affixes

[edit]

Prefixes

[edit]

Verbal prefixes are used only in the independent order, although some forms of the independent order lack a prefix. There are three of them:/n-/ (first person),/k-/ (second person), and/w-/ (third person). If a stem has an underlying initial vowel, a/t/ is inserted after the prefix, and before this and other stem-initial consonants a/ə/ is inserted. Sometimes, this/ə/ contracts with a stem-initial/wə/ to/o·/ except when the/o·/ would be phonetically shortened via rule U-4a. Examples include:/ntá·mwi/ ('I get up from lying') versus/á·mwi·(w)/ ('he gets up'). Two roots with initial/t/ extend the syllable with/-ən/ when adding prefixes; these roots are/tal-/ ('there') and/tax-/ ('so many'), e.g.náni ntəntala·wsí·ne·n ('that is where we live [our lives]') from the animate intransitive stem/tala·wəsi·/.[31]

Prefixes are mutually exclusive and are selected based on the following rule: if one of the participants is second person, the second person prefix is used; if not, if one of the participants is the first person, then the first person prefix is used; if none of these applies, other forms, if they take a prefix, take the third person prefix. This is the well-known Algonquian 2-1-3 precedence rule.[32]

Suffixes

[edit]

Suffixes are grouped into eight positional classes. These are:[31]

  • Theme
    1. Theme signs
  • Thematic affixes
  • Diminutive and pejorative
    1. Obviative and plural
    2. Negative; imperative modes
  • Desinences (inflectional suffixes)
    1. Central endings
    2. Aspect
    3. Peripheral endings
    4. Mode
First position
[edit]

The first position (theme signs) is filled only for transitive verbs and help describe the relationship between the two participants by indicating which is theagent and which is theobject. The direct and inverse theme signs indicate the direction of the verb along a spectrum what might be called distance. From least to most distant the participants are: (1) first or second; (2) indefinite (only as subject); (3) proximate third person; (4) obviative third person; (5) farther obviative third person; (6) inanimate (subject only). If the subject is less distant than the object, the direct theme is used; if the subject is more distant, the inverse signs are used. After transitive animate (TA) verb stems appear one of the four following theme signs:

  • Theme 1:1/-a·/ ~1/-∅/; makesdirect forms
  • Theme 2:1/-əkw/ ~1/-əke·/; makesinverse forms
  • Theme 3:1/-i·/; makesfirst person object forms
  • Theme 4:1/-əl/; makessecond person forms

For transitive inanimate (TI) verbs, there appears the theme sign pertaining to the appropriate TI class:

  • Class 1a:1/-am/
  • Class 1b:1/-əm/
  • Class 2:1/-o·/ ~1/-aw/
  • Class 3: no theme sign

For Class 2 TI theme signs, in certain derivational categories, the theme sign is retained even when the thematic element is dropped. The contrast between both categories is sharper in the Central Algonquian languages, whose theme sign has a more complex series of alternants.[33]

Second position
[edit]

The second position consists ofdiminutives in2/-tī/ andpejoratives in2/-šī/ (in both of these, the/-ī/ is unstable.) An example of a diminutive is/pé·t·o/ ('the little one comes'), contrasted with the pejorative/pé·šo/ ('the undesirable one came'). The use of/t/ in the formation of diminutives seems to be an innovation of Unami, as many other Algonquian languages use*/s/ or*/ʃ/ (in fact, the diminutive of Munsee is/-šī/).

Third position
[edit]

In position three are the affixes3/-lī/, which marks the obviative third person, and3/-h(ə)tī/, which marks the plural.

Fourth position
[edit]

Position four contains the negative affix4/-(ō)w(ī)/ and prohibitive imperative and future imperative forms, which have complex series of alternants.

Fifth position
[edit]

Position five contains the central endings which index the central participant of each form, except those using TA theme signs1/-i·/ and1/-əl/;

Sixth position
[edit]

Position six contains the affix endings:/-pan/ marks the preterite, and/sa/ ~/shan/ mark the present.

Seventh position
[edit]

Position seven contains peripheral endings, which are used to mark the nominal category of some 3rd person participants in forms in the independent and conjunct (but not imperative) orders.

Eighth position
[edit]

Position eight reflects the subjunctive, prohibitive, and future modes.

Syntax

[edit]

Unami is, like manyAlgonquian languages, polysynthetic and highly agglutinative. This means that most of the information is encoded in the verb (sometimes with whole words being incorporated into the stem), making word order more fluid than in English. Syntax is one of the least studied aspects of the Unami language; there is much more data on morphology, because of an especial focus on reconstructing Proto-Algonquian.

Some examples of complex sentences in Unami include:[34]

  • ta heč tɔllí·ksi·n ne·k ma·nšá·p·iyak? ('what color are those beads?')
  • še· lah ni e·k·aɔ́·kwe [tali-]phɔkhakéhɔ·n ní·ša awé·ni·k ('over there under the trees two people were buried')
  • mi·mə́nsak šokw təli-ahi-pe·nháto·n ('but the children's footprints are numerous')
  • na hont náni tənnə́mən níkahke pi·lae·č·əč·ínka ('then that's what those boys did')

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^abChambers, Steve."The vanishing voice of the Lenape."[usurped]Star-Ledger. 17 Nov 2002. Retrieved 3 Dec 2022, archived from the original on 4 Jun 2019
  2. ^Pentland, David, 1992, p. 15; Goddard, Ives, 1996, p. 5
  3. ^Hammarström, Harald; Forkel, Robert; Haspelmath, Martin; Bank, Sebastian (2022-05-24)."Delawaran".Glottolog.Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Archived from the original on 2022-10-30. Retrieved2022-10-29.
  4. ^Online Lenape Talking Dictionary, "Unami (a group of the Lenape or Delaware Indians)",[1]
  5. ^Kraft, Herbert C.,The Lenape-Delaware Indian Heritage: 10,000 BC to AD 2000, Lenape Books, p.9, n.2
  6. ^Lenapehoking MapArchived 2006-01-13 at theWayback Machine, showing approximate locations of Lenape Indian Bands, Lenape Lifeways website (accessed December 16, 2007)
  7. ^abGoddard, Ives. 1996. "Introduction." Ives Goddard, ed., The Handbook of North American Indians, Volume 17. Languages, pp. 1-16. Washington, D.C.: The Smithsonian Institution.
  8. ^Goddard, Ives, 1978
  9. ^Pentland, David, 1982
  10. ^Goddard, Ives. 1982. "Munsee historical phonology." International Journal of American Linguistics 48: 16-48.
  11. ^Goddard, Ives, 1982; Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 11,
  12. ^"Lenape Language Preservation Project."Delaware Tribe of Indians. Retrieved 8 Dec 2013.
  13. ^Heckewelder, Rev. John:History, Manners, and Customs of the Indian Nations Who Once Inhabited Pennsylvania and the Neighboring States, pages 353-432. Facsimile reprint by Heritage Books, Bowie, MD, 1990,ISBN 1-55613-411-8
  14. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. vi; for a consonant chart see Ives Goddard, 1997, pp. 43-98.
  15. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 22, xii (rule U-6); p. 26 (rule U-27)
  16. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. viii
  17. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 28
  18. ^Goddard, Ives, 1997: 45
  19. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 21
  20. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. xv
  21. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 12, 22
  22. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 18-19
  23. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 21, 130
  24. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 12-13
  25. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 20-26
  26. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 31-34
  27. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 30–31
  28. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 47
  29. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 29
  30. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 167-191
  31. ^abGoddard, Ives, 1979, p. 76
  32. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 78
  33. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 73
  34. ^Goddard, Ives, 1979

References

[edit]

External links

[edit]
Algonquian
Arapahoan
Cree–Montagnais–Naskapi
Cree
Others
Eastern Algonquian
Southern New England
Delawaran
Nanticockan
Others
Mesquakie–Sauk–Kickapoo
OjibwaPotawatomi
Ojibwa
Potawatomi
Others
Others
Uncertain
Proto-languages
1Creole/Pidgin/Mixed language • Italics indicateextinct languages
Languages initalics are extinct.
English
Dialects ofAmerican English
Oral Indigenous
languages
Families
Algic
Austronesian
Caddoan
Chinookan
Chumashan
Dené(–
Yeniseian?)
Eskaleut
Iroquoian
Kalapuyan
Keresan
  • Cochiti Pueblo
  • San Felipe–Santo Domingo
  • Zia–Santa Ana Pueblos
  • Western Keres
  • Acoma Pueblo
  • Laguna Pueblo
Maiduan
Muskogean
Palaihnihan
Pakawan
Plateau Penutian
Pomoan
Salishan
Siouan
Tanoan
Tsimshianic
Uto-Aztecan
Wakashan
Wintuan
Yuk-Utian
Yuman–
Cochimí
Others
Isolates
Mixed or trade
Languages
Manual Indigenous
languages
Hand Talk
  • Anishinaabe Sign Language
  • Blackfoot Sign Language
  • Cheyenne Sign Language
  • Cree Sign Language
  • Navajo Sign Language
Isolates
Oral settler
languages
French
German
Spanish
Creole andmixed languages
Manual settler
languages
Francosign
BANZSL
Kentish
Isolates
Immigrant languages
(number of speakers
in 2021 in millions)
Italics indicate extinct languages
Indigenous language
Immigrant languages
Sign languages
Italics indicate extinct languages
Indigenous languages
Immigrant
Sign languages
Italics indicate extinct languages * indicates extinct language in Oklahoma but still spoken elsewhere
Indigenous
Sign languages
Non-Indigenous
Native Americans in Maryland
Historical and present tribes
Historical languages
Present territories
Historic figures
Historical communities
Precontact peoples
Precontact communities
Other precontact sites
Institutions
Other topics
Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Unami_language&oldid=1279179320"
Categories:
Hidden categories:

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp