Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Jump to content
WikipediaThe Free Encyclopedia
Search

Primate

Featured article
Page semi-protected
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected fromPrimates)
Order of mammals

This article is about the type of animal. For other uses, seePrimate (disambiguation).

Primates
Scientific classificationEdit this classification
Domain:Eukaryota
Kingdom:Animalia
Phylum:Chordata
Class:Mammalia
Mirorder:Primatomorpha
Order:Primates
Linnaeus, 1758[1]
Suborders
Range and density of non-human primates.
Synonyms

Plesiadapiformes (cladistically including crown primates[2])

Primates is anorder ofmammals, which is further divided into thestrepsirrhines, which includelemurs,galagos, andlorisids; and thehaplorhines, which includetarsiers andsimians (monkeys andapes). Primates arose 74–63 million years ago first from smallterrestrial mammals, which adapted for life intropical forests: many primate characteristics represent adaptations to the challenging environment amongtree tops, including largebrain sizes,binocular vision,color vision,vocalizations,shoulder girdles allowing a large degree of movement in theupper limbs, andopposable thumbs (in most but not all) that enable bettergrasping anddexterity. Primates range in size fromMadame Berthe's mouse lemur, which weighs 30 g (1 oz), to theeastern gorilla, weighing over 200 kg (440 lb). There are 376–524species of living primates, depending on which classification is used. New primate species continue to be discovered: over 25 species were described in the 2000s, 36 in the 2010s, andsix in the 2020s.

Primates have largebrains (relative to body size) compared to other mammals, as well as an increased reliance onvisual acuity at the expense of thesense of smell, which is the dominant sensory system in most mammals. These features are more developed in monkeys and apes, and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs. Some primates, includinggorillas,humans andbaboons, are primarily ground-dwelling rather than arboreal, but all species have adaptations for climbing trees.Arboreal locomotion techniques used include leaping from tree to tree and swinging between branches of trees (brachiation); terrestrial locomotion techniques include walking on twohindlimbs (bipedalism) and modified walking on four limbs (quadrupedalism) viaknuckle-walking.

Primates are among the mostsocial of all animals, forming pairs or family groups, uni-male harems, and multi-male/multi-female groups. Non-human primates have at least four types ofsocial systems, many defined by the amount of movement by adolescent females between groups. Primates have slower rates of development than other similarly sized mammals, reach maturity later, and have longer lifespans. Primates are also the mostcognitively advanced animals, with humans (genusHomo) capable of creating complexlanguages and sophisticatedcivilizations, and non-human primates are recorded touse tools. They may communicate using facial and hand gestures, smells and vocalizations.

Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create opportunities for the transmission ofzoonotic diseases, especially virus diseases includingherpes,measles,ebola,rabies andhepatitis. Thousands of non-human primates are used in research around the world because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction. Common threats includedeforestation,forest fragmentation,monkey drives, and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing for agriculture most threatens primates.

Etymology

The English nameprimates is derived fromOld French or Frenchprimat, from a noun use of Latinprimat-, fromprimus ('prime, first rank').[3] The name was given byCarl Linnaeus because he thought this the "highest" order of animals.[4] The relationships among the different groups of primates were not clearly understood until relatively recently, so the commonly used terms are somewhat confused. For example,ape has been used either as an alternative formonkey or for any tailless, relatively human-like primate.[5][6]

SirWilfrid Le Gros Clark was one of theprimatologists who developed the idea of trends in primate evolution and the methodology of arranging the living members of an order into an "ascending series" leading to humans.[7] Commonly used names for groups of primates such asprosimians,monkeys,lesser apes, andgreat apes reflect this methodology. According to our current understanding of the evolutionary history of the primates, several of these groups areparaphyletic, or rather they do not include all the descendants of a common ancestor.[8]

In contrast with Clark's methodology, modern classifications typically identify (or name) only those groupings that aremonophyletic; that is, such a named group includesall the descendants of the group's common ancestor.[9]

All groups with scientific names areclades, or monophyletic groups, and the sequence of scientific classification reflects the evolutionary history of the related lineages. Groups that are traditionally named are shown on the right; they form an "ascending series" (per Clark, see above), and several groups are paraphyletic:

  • Prosimians contain two monophyletic groups (the suborder Strepsirrhini, or lemurs, lorises and allies, as well as the tarsiers of the suborder Haplorhini); it is a paraphyletic grouping because it excludes the Simiiformes, which also are descendants of the common ancestor Primates.
  • Monkeys comprise two monophyletic groups, New World monkeys and Old World monkeys, but is paraphyletic because it excludes hominoids, superfamily Hominoidea, also descendants of the common ancestor Simiiformes.
  • Apes as a whole, and thegreat apes, are paraphyletic if the terms are used such that they exclude humans.

Thus, the members of the two sets of groups, and hence names, do not match, which causes problems in relating scientific names to common (usually traditional) names. Consider the superfamily Hominoidea: In terms of the common names on the right, this group consists of apes and humans and there is no single common name for all the members of the group. One remedy is to create a new common name, in this casehominoids. Another possibility is to expand the use of one of the traditional names. For example, in his 2005 book, thevertebratepalaeontologist Benton wrote, "The apes, Hominoidea, today include thegibbons andorangutan ... thegorilla andchimpanzee ... andhumans";[10] thereby Benton was usingapes to mean hominoids. In that case, the group heretofore calledapes must now be identified as the non-human apes.

As of 2021[update], there is no consensus as to whether to accept traditional (that is, common), but paraphyletic, names or to use monophyletic names only; or to use 'new' common names or adaptations of old ones. Both competing approaches can be found in biological sources, often in the same work, and sometimes by the same author. Thus, Benton definesapes to include humans, then he repeatedly usesape-like to mean 'like an ape rather than a human'; and when discussing the reaction of others to a new fossil he writes of "claims thatOrrorin ... was an ape rather than a human".[11]

Classification of living primates

Order Primates was established byCarl Linnaeus in 1758, in thetenth edition of his bookSystema Naturae,[12] for the generaHomo (humans),Simia (other apes and monkeys),Lemur (prosimians) andVespertilio (bats). In the first edition of the same book (1735), he had used the nameAnthropomorpha forHomo,Simia andBradypus (sloths).[13] In 1839,Henri Marie Ducrotay de Blainville, following Linnaeus and aping his nomenclature, established the ordersSecundates (including the subordersChiroptera,Insectivora andCarnivora),Tertiates (orGlires) andQuaternates (includingGravigrada,Pachydermata andRuminantia),[14] but these new taxa were not accepted.

Before Anderson and Jones introduced the classification of Strepsirrhini and Haplorhini in 1984,[15] (followed by McKenna and Bell's 1997 workClassification of Mammals: Above the species level),[16] Primates was divided into two superfamilies:Prosimii andAnthropoidea.[17] Prosimii included all of theprosimians: Strepsirrhini plus thetarsiers. Anthropoidea contained all of thesimians.

Thecladogram below shows one possible classification sequence of the living primates:[18][19] groups that use common (traditional) names are shown on the right.

Primatomorpha

Dermoptera

Primates
Strepsirrhini
Lemuriformes[a]

lemurs (superfamily Lemuroidea)

lorises and allies (superfamily Lorisoidea)

Haplorhini
Tarsiiformes

tarsiers (superfamily Tarsioidea)

Simiiformes

New World monkeys (parvorder Platyrrhini)

Catarrhini

Old World monkeys (superfamily Cercopithecoidea)

Hominoidea

gibbons (family Hylobatidae)

Hominidae

orangutans (subfamily Ponginae)

Homininae

gorillas (tribe Gorillini)

Hominini
prosimians
monkeys
lesser apes
great apes

Phylogeny and genetics

Euarchontoglires
Glires

Rodentia (rodents)

Lagomorpha (rabbits, hares, pikas)

Euarchonta

Scandentia (treeshrews)

Primatomorpha

Dermoptera (colugos)

Primates

Plesiadapiformes

crown primates

Order Primates is part of the cladeEuarchontoglires, which is nested within the cladeEutheria of ClassMammalia. Recent molecular genetic research on primates,colugos, andtreeshrews has shown that the two species of colugos are more closely related to primates than to treeshrews,[23] even though treeshrews were at one time considered primates.[24] These three orders make up thecladeEuarchonta. The combination of this clade with the cladeGlires (composed ofRodentia andLagomorpha) forms the clade Euarchontoglires. Variously, both Euarchonta and Euarchontoglires are ranked as superorders. Some scientists consider Dermoptera to be a suborder of Primates and use the suborder Euprimates for the "true" primates.[25]

Evolutionary history

Further information:Evolution of primates

The primate lineage is thought to go back at least near theCretaceous–Paleogene boundary or around 74–63 (mya).[26][27][28][29][30] The earliest possible primate/proto-primate may bePurgatorius, which dates back toEarly Paleocene of North America ~66mya.[31][32] The oldest known primates from the fossil record date to the Late Paleocene of Africa, c.57 mya (Altiatlasius)[33] or the Paleocene-Eocene transition in the northern continents, c. 55 mya (Cantius,Donrussellia,Altanius,Plesiadapis andTeilhardina).[34][35][31] Other studies, including molecular clock studies, have estimated the origin of the primate branch to have been in the mid-Cretaceous period, around 85 mya.[36][37][38]

By moderncladistic reckoning, the order Primates ismonophyletic. The suborderStrepsirrhini, the "wet-nosed" primates, is generally thought to have split off from the primitive primate line about 63 mya,[39] although earlier dates are also supported.[40] The seven strepsirrhine families are the five relatedlemur families and the two remaining families that include thelorisids and thegalagos.[1][41] Older classification schemes wrapLepilemuridae intoLemuridae andGalagidae intoLorisidae, yielding a four-one family distribution instead of five-two as presented here.[1] During theEocene, most of the northern continents were dominated by two groups, theadapiforms and theomomyids.[42][43] The former are considered members of Strepsirrhini, but did not have atoothcomb like modern lemurs; recent analysis has demonstrated thatDarwinius masillae fits into this grouping.[44] The latter was closely related to tarsiers, monkeys, and apes. How these two groups relate to extant primates is unclear. Omomyids perished about 30 mya,[43] while adapiforms survived until about 10 mya.[45]

According to genetic studies, the lemurs of Madagascar diverged from the lorisoids approximately 75 mya.[40] These studies, as well as chromosomal and molecular evidence, also show that lemurs are more closely related to each other than to other strepsirrhine primates.[40][46] However, Madagascar split from Africa 160 mya and from India 90 mya.[47] To account for these facts, a founding lemur population of a few individuals is thought to have reached Madagascar from Africa via a singlerafting event between 50 and 80 mya.[40][46][47] Other colonization options have been suggested, such as multiple colonizations from Africa and India,[42] but none are supported by the genetic and molecular evidence.[40]

Common brown lemur, astrepsirrhine primate

Until recently, theaye-aye has been difficult to place within Strepsirrhini.[1] Theories had been proposed that its family, Daubentoniidae, was either a lemuriform primate (meaning its ancestors split from the lemur line more recently than lemurs and lorises split) or a sister group to all the other strepsirrhines. In 2008, the aye-aye family was confirmed to be most closely related to the other Malagasy lemurs, likely having descended from the same ancestral population that colonized the island.[40]

SuborderHaplorhini, the simple-nosed or "dry-nosed" primates, is composed of two sister clades.[1]Prosimian tarsiers in the family Tarsiidae (monotypic in its own infraorder Tarsiiformes), represent the mostbasal division, originating about 58 mya.[48][49] The earliest known haplorhine skeleton, that of 55 MA old tarsier-likeArchicebus, was found in central China,[50] supporting an already suspected Asian origin for the group.[51] The infraorderSimiiformes (simian primates, consisting of monkeys and apes) emerged about 40 mya,[43] possibly also in Asia; if so, theydispersed across theTethys Sea from Asia to Africa soon afterwards.[52] There are two simian clades, bothparvorders:Catarrhini, which developed in Africa, consisting ofOld World monkeys, humans and the other apes, and Platyrrhini, which developed in South America, consisting ofNew World monkeys.[1] A third clade, which included theeosimiids, developed in Asia, but became extinct millions of years ago.[53]

As in the case of lemurs, the origin of New World monkeys is unclear. Molecular studies of concatenated nuclear sequences have yielded a widely varying estimated date of divergence between platyrrhines and catarrhines, ranging from 33 to 70 mya, while studies based on mitochondrial sequences produce a narrower range of 35 to 43 mya.[35][54] The anthropoid primates possibly traversed the Atlantic Ocean from Africa to South America during theEocene byisland hopping, facilitated byAtlantic Ocean ridges and a lowered sea level.[42] Alternatively, a singlerafting event may explain this transoceanic colonization. Due tocontinental drift, the Atlantic Ocean was not nearly as wide at the time as it is today.[42] Research suggests that a small 1 kg (2.2 lb) primate could have survived 13 days on a raft of vegetation.[55] Given estimated current and wind speeds, this would have provided enough time to make the voyage between the continents.

Emperor tamarin, aNew World monkey

Apes and monkeys spread from Africa into Europe and Asia starting in theMiocene.[56] Soon after, the lorises and tarsiers made the same journey. The first hominin fossils were discovered in northern Africa and date back 5–8 mya.[43] Old World monkeys disappeared from Europe about 1.8 mya.[57] Molecular and fossil studies generally show that modern humans originated in Africa 100,000–200,000 years ago.[58]

Although primates are well studied in comparison to other animal groups, several new species havebeen discovered recently, and genetic tests have revealed previously unrecognised species in known populations.Primate Taxonomy listed about 350 species of primates in 2001;[19] the author,Colin Groves, increased that number to 376 for his contribution to the third edition ofMammal Species of the World (MSW3).[1] However, publications since the taxonomy in MSW3 was compiled in 2003 have pushed the number to 522 species, or 708 including subspecies.[59]

Hybrids

Primatehybrids usually arise in captivity,[60] but there have also been examples in the wild.[61][62] Hybridization occurs where two species' range overlap to formhybrid zones; hybrids may be created by humans when animals are placed in zoos or due to environmental pressures such as predation.[61] Intergeneric hybridizations, hybrids of different genera, have also been found in the wild. Although they belong to genera that have been distinct for several million years, interbreeding still occurs between thegelada and thehamadryas baboon.[63]

Clones

On 24 January 2018, scientists in China reported in the journalCell the creation of twocrab-eating macaqueclones, namedZhong Zhong andHua Hua, using thecomplex DNA transfer method that producedDolly the sheep, for the first time.[64][65][66][67][68]

Anatomy and physiology

Head

Primate skulls showingpostorbital bar, and increasing brain sizes

The primate skull has a large, domedcranium, which is particularly prominent inanthropoids. The cranium protects the large brain, a distinguishing characteristic of this group.[69] The endocranial volume (the volume within the skull) is three times greater inhumans than in the greatest nonhuman primate, reflecting a larger brain size.[70] The mean endocranial volume is 1,201 cubic centimeters in humans, 469 cm3 ingorillas, 400 cm3 inchimpanzees and 397 cm3 inorangutans.[70] The primary evolutionary trend of primates has been the elaboration of the brain, in particular theneocortex (a part of thecerebral cortex), which is involved withsensory perception, generation ofmotor commands, spatial reasoning,conscious thought and, in humans,language.[71] While other mammals rely heavily on theirsense of smell, the arboreal life of primates has led to atactile,visually dominant sensory system,[71] a reduction in the olfactory region of the brain and increasingly complex social behavior.[72] The visual acuity ofhumans and otherhominids is exceptional; they have the mostacute vision known among all vertebrates, with the exception of certain species ofpredatory birds.[73][74]

Primates have forward-facing eyes on the front of the skull;binocular vision allows accurate distance perception, useful for thebrachiating ancestors of all great apes.[69] Abony ridge above the eye sockets reinforces weaker bones in the face, which are put under strain during chewing.Strepsirrhines have apostorbital bar, a bone around the eye socket, to protect their eyes; in contrast, the higher primates,haplorhines, have evolved fully enclosed sockets.[75]

An 1893 drawing of the hands and feet of various primates

Primates show an evolutionary trend towards a reducedsnout.[76] Technically, Old World monkeys are distinguished from New World monkeys by the structure of the nose, and from apes by thearrangement of their teeth.[72] In New World monkeys, the nostrils face sideways; in Old World monkeys, they face downwards.[72] Dental pattern in primates vary considerably; although some have lost most of theirincisors, all retain at least one lower incisor.[72] In most strepsirrhines, the lower incisors form atoothcomb, which is used in grooming and sometimes foraging.[72][77] Old World monkeys have eightpremolars, compared with 12 in New World monkeys. The Old World species are divided into apes and monkeys depending on the number ofcusps on theirmolars: monkeys have four, apes have five[72] - although humans may have four or five.[78] The main hominid molar cusp (hypocone) evolved in early primate history, while the cusp of the corresponding primitive lower molar (paraconid) was lost. Prosimians are distinguished by their immobilized upper lips, the moist tip of their noses and forward-facing lower front teeth.

Body

Vervet hindfoot showing fingerprint ridges on the sole

Primates generally have five digits on each limb (pentadactyly), with a characteristic type of keratinfingernail on the end of each finger and toe. The bottom sides of the hands and feet havesensitive pads on thefingertips. Most haveopposable thumbs, a characteristic primate feature most developed inhumans, though not limited to this order (opossums andkoalas, for example, also have them).[69] Thumbs allow some species to usetools. In primates, the combination of opposing thumbs, short fingernails (rather than claws) and long, inward-closing fingers is arelict of the ancestral practice of gripping branches, and has, in part, allowed some species to developbrachiation (swinging by the arms from tree limb to tree limb) as a significant means of locomotion.Prosimians have clawlike nails on the second toe of each foot, calledtoilet-claws, which they use for grooming.[69]

The primatecollar bone is a prominent element of thepectoral girdle; this allows theshoulder joint broad mobility.[76] Compared to Old World monkeys, apes have more mobile shoulder joints and arms due to the dorsal position of thescapula, broad ribcages that are flatter front-to-back, a shorter, less mobile spine, and with lowervertebrae greatly reduced - resulting in tail loss in some species.[6]Prehensile tails are found in the New Worldatelids, including thehowler,spider,woolly spider,woolly monkeys; and incapuchins.[79][80] Male primates have alow-hanging penis and testes descended into a scrotum.[81][77]

Sexual dimorphism

Main article:Sexual dimorphism in non-human primates
Distinct sexual size dimorphism can be seen between the male and female gorilla.

Sexual dimorphism is often exhibited insimians, though to a greater degree in Old World species (apes and some monkeys) than New World species. Recent studies involve comparing DNA to examine both the variation in the expression of the dimorphism among primates and the fundamental causes of sexual dimorphism. Primates usually have dimorphism inbody mass[82][83] and canine tooth size[84][85] along withpelage and skin color.[86] The dimorphism can be attributed to and affected by different factors, includingmating system,[87] size,[87] habitat and diet.[88]

Comparative analyses have generated a more complete understanding of the relationship betweensexual selection,natural selection, and mating systems in primates. Studies have shown that dimorphism is the product of changes in both male and female traits.[89] Ontogenetic scaling, where relative extension of a common growth trajectory occurs, may give some insight into the relationship between sexual dimorphism and growth patterns.[90] Some evidence from the fossil record suggests that there wasconvergent evolution of dimorphism, and some extincthominids probably had greater dimorphism than any living primate.[89]

Locomotion

Diademed sifaka, a lemur that is a vertical clinger and leaper

Primate species move bybrachiation,bipedalism,leaping, arboreal and terrestrialquadrupedalism,climbing,knuckle-walking or by a combination of these methods. Several prosimians are primarily vertical clingers and leapers. These include manybushbabies, allindriids (i.e.,sifakas,avahis andindris),sportive lemurs, and alltarsiers.[91] Other prosimians are arboreal quadrupeds and climbers. Some are also terrestrial quadrupeds, while some are leapers. Most monkeys are both arboreal and terrestrial quadrupeds and climbers.Gibbons,muriquis andspider monkeys all brachiate extensively,[57] with gibbons sometimes doing so in remarkably acrobatic fashion.Woolly monkeys also brachiate at times.[92]Orangutans use a similar form of locomotion called quadramanous climbing, in which they use their arms and legs to carry their heavy bodies through the trees.[57]Chimpanzees andgorillas knuckle walk,[57] and can move bipedally for short distances. Although numerous species, such asaustralopithecines andearly hominids, have exhibited fully bipedal locomotion, humans are the only extant species with this trait.[93]

Vision

Thetapetum lucidum of anorthern greater galago, typical of prosimians, reflects the light of the photographer's flash.

Theevolution of color vision in primates is unique among mosteutherian mammals. While the remotevertebrate ancestors of the primates possessedthree color vision (trichromaticism), thenocturnal,warm-blooded, mammalian ancestors lost one of three cones in theretina during theMesozoic era. Fish,reptiles and birds are therefore trichromatic ortetrachromatic, while all mammals, with the exception of some primates andmarsupials,[94] are dichromats ormonochromats (totally color blind).[77] Nocturnal primates, such as thenight monkeys andbush babies, are often monochromatic. Catarrhines are routinely trichromatic due to agene duplication of the red-greenopsin gene at the base of their lineage, 30 to 40 million years ago.[77][95] Platyrrhines, on the other hand, are trichromatic in a few cases only.[96] Specifically, individual females must beheterozygous for twoalleles of the opsin gene (red and green) located on the samelocus of theX chromosome.[77] Males, therefore, can only be dichromatic, while females can be either dichromatic or trichromatic. Color vision in strepsirrhines is not as well understood; however, research indicates a range of color vision similar to that found in platyrrhines.[77]

Like catarrhines, howler monkeys (a family of platyrrhines) show routine trichromatism that has been traced to an evolutionarily recentgene duplication.[97] Howler monkeys are one of the most specialized leaf-eaters of the New World monkeys; fruits are not a major part of their diets,[92] and the type of leaves they prefer to consume (young, nutritive, and digestible) are detectable only by a red-green signal. Field work exploring the dietary preferences of howler monkeys suggests that routine trichromaticism was selected by environment.[96]

Behavior

Social systems

Richard Wrangham stated thatsocial systems of primates are best classified by the amount of movement by females occurring between groups.[98] He proposed four categories:

  • Female transfer systems – females move away from the group in which they were born. Females of a group will not be closely related whereas males will have remained with their natal groups, and this close association may be influential in social behavior. The groups formed are generally quite small.[98] This organization can be seen inchimpanzees, where the males, who are typically related, will cooperate in defense of the group's territory.[99] Evidence of this social system has also been found amongNeanderthal remains inSpain[100] and in remains ofAustralopithecus andParanthropus robustus groups in southern Africa.[101][102] Among New World Monkeys,spider monkeys andmuriquis use this system.[103]
A social huddle ofring-tailed lemurs. The two individuals on the right exposing their white ventral surface are sunning themselves.
  • Male transfer systems – while the females remain in their natal groups, the males will emigrate as adolescents. Group sizes are usually larger.[98] This system is common among thering-tailed lemur,capuchin monkeys andcercopithecine monkeys.[57]
  • Monogamous species – a male–female bond, sometimes accompanied by a juvenile offspring. There is shared responsibility of parental care and territorial defense. The offspring leaves the parents' territory during adolescence.[98]Indri,lariang tarsiers,Callitrichidae monkeys and gibbons use this system, although "monogamy" in this context does not necessarily mean absolute sexual fidelity.[104][105] These species do not live in larger groups.
  • Solitary species – males and females live in overlapping home ranges.[98][105] This type of organization is found in lorises, galagos,mouse lemurs, aye-ayes and orangutans.[105]

Other systems are known to occur as well. For example, withhowler monkeys andgorillas both the males and females typically transfer from their natal group on reaching sexual maturity, resulting in groups in which neither the males nor females are typically related.[92][106] Some prosimians,colobine monkeys andcallitrichid monkeys also use this system.[57]

The transfer of females or males from their native group is likely an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding.[107] An analysis of breeding records of captive primate colonies representing numerous different species indicates that the infant mortality of inbred young is generally higher than that of non-inbred young.[107][108] This effect of inbreeding on infant mortality is probably largely a result of increased expression of deleterious recessive alleles (seeInbreeding depression).

Chimpanzees are social great apes.

PrimatologistJane Goodall, who studied in theGombe Stream National Park, notedfission-fusion societies in chimpanzees.[109] There isfission when the main group splits up to forage during the day, thenfusion when the group returns at night to sleep as a group. This social structure can also be observed in thehamadryas baboon,[110]spider monkeys[92] and thebonobo.[110] Thegelada has a similar social structure in which many smaller groups come together to form temporary herds of up to 600 monkeys.[110]Humans also form fission-fusion societies. In hunter-gatherer societies, humans form groups which are made up of several individuals that may split up to obtain different resources.[111]

These social systems are affected by three main ecological factors: distribution of resources,group size, andpredation.[112] Within a social group there is a balance between cooperation and competition. Cooperative behaviors in many primates species includesocial grooming (removingskin parasites and cleaning wounds), food sharing, and collective defense against predators or of a territory. Aggressive behaviors often signal competition for food, sleeping sites or mates. Aggression is also used in establishingdominance hierarchies.[112][113]

In November 2023, scientists reported, for the first time, evidence that groups of primates, particularlybonobos, are capable of cooperating with each other.[114][115]

Interspecific associations

Several species of primates are known to associate in the wild. Some of these associations have been extensively studied. In theTai Forest of Africa, several species coordinate anti-predator behavior. These include theDiana monkey,Campbell's mona monkey,lesser spot-nosed monkey,western red colobus,king colobus (westernblack and white colobus), andsooty mangabey, which coordinate anti-predator alarm calls.[116] Among the predators of these monkeys is thecommon chimpanzee.[117]

Thered-tailed monkey associates with several species, including the western red colobus,blue monkey,Wolf's mona monkey,mantled guereza,black crested mangabey andAllen's swamp monkey.[110] Several of these species are preyed upon by the common chimpanzee.[118]

In South America,squirrel monkeys associate withcapuchin monkeys.[119] This may have more to do with foraging benefits to the squirrel monkeys than anti-predation benefits.[119]

Mating systems

Twocommon marmosets, the species lives in monogamous pairs
Gelada harem: one male and multiple females

Themating systems of primates vary betweenmonogamy,polyandry,polygyny andpolygynandry. In monogamous species, adult males and females form long-lastingpair bonds. Compared to other systems, there is little competition for mating rights and males and females tend to be similar in size. Polyandry, which involves groups consisting of single females mating with multiple males, may arise as a secondary mating system in monogamous species. In thebrown-mantled tamarin, a female may breeding with one or two males. Polyandry may have developed due to the high frequency of twin births, which require more help in raising.[105]

Polygynous species include gorillas,Hanuman langurs, geladas, hamadryas baboons,proboscis monkeys, andgolden snub-nosed monkeys, and consists of one male mating with multiple females within aharem or one-male unit. Sexual dimorphism tends to be higher in these species and males may also develop prominentsecondary sex characteristics. In the patriarchal hamadryas baboon, the males aggressively herd females into their groups and violently discipline those that wander. By contrast, in gelada society, which is based on female kinship, a male is dependent on the support of the females in his unit and cannot impose on them. Polygynous males must defend their harems from rivals, who may try to take over.[105]

In some species, such as ring-tailed lemurs,sifakas,macaques, mostbaboons,mangabeys,squirrel monkeys,woolly monkeys, spider monkeys,woolly spider monkeys, chimpanzees and bonobos, both males and females mate with multiple partners. Polygynandry occurs inmultimale-multifemale groups, and since females mate many times before conception, males have large testicles forsperm competition. Males may exist in adominance hierarchy and those at the top will try to monopolize access to the females. Consortships may occur in some species but these are short-term. In solitary-living species, males and females mate with partners whose home ranges they overlap with. This is known as a 'dispersed' mating system.[105]

Genetic evidence indicates that humans were predominantlypolygynous for most of their existence as a species, but that this began to shift during the Neolithic, whenmonogamy started becoming widespread concomitantly with the transition from nomadic to sedentary societies.[120] Most modern human societies consist of monogamousmarriages, but allow for polygyny, particularly for those of a high status.[105]

Sexual behavior

Bonobos mating,Jacksonville Zoo and Gardens.

Female primates may signal to the male their receptiveness though various displays including eye-contact, tongue-clicking and presenting of the rump. Female lemurs, lorises and galagos will position themselves in thelordosis pose while female chimpanzees, bonobos and some Old World monkeys developsexual swellings on the rump. Copulation in primates typically involves the males mounting the females from behind, as with most mammals. Belly-to-belly copulation has been recorded in apes, both gibbons and the great apes. Humansex positions are modifications of these two positions.[121]

Primates may egage in sexual activity as part of social bonding; includinghomosexual behaviour.[121] Such behavior play an important role in bonobo society in particular. female bonobos engage in mutual genital-rubbing behavior, possibly to bond socially with each other, thus forming a female nucleus of bonobo society. The bonding among females enables them to dominate most of the males.[122]

Life history

Acrab-eating macaque breastfeeding her baby

Primates have slower rates of development than other mammals. All primate infants arebreastfed by their mothers (with the exception of some human cultures and various zoo raised primates which are fed formula) and rely on them for grooming and transportation. In some species, infants are protected and transported by males in the group, particularly males who may be their fathers. Other relatives of the infant, such as siblings and aunts, may participate in its care as well. Most primate mothers ceaseovulation while breastfeeding an infant; once the infant isweaned the mother can reproduce again. This often leads to weaning conflict with infants who attempt to continue breastfeeding.[57]

Infanticide is common in polygynous species such asgray langurs and gorillas. Adult males may kill dependent offspring that are not theirs so the female will return to estrus and thus they can sire offspring of their own. Social monogamy in some species may have evolved to combat this behavior.[123] Polygynandry may also lessen the risk of infanticide since paternity becomes uncertain.[124]

Primates have a longerjuvenile period between weaning and sexual maturity than other mammals of similar size.[57] Some primates such asgalagos andNew World monkeys use tree-holes fornesting, and park juveniles in leafy patches while foraging. Other primates follow a strategy of "riding", i.e. carrying individuals on the body while feeding. Adults may construct or use nesting sites, sometimes accompanied by juveniles, for the purpose of resting, a behavior which has developed secondarily in the great apes.[125][126] During the juvenile period, primates are more susceptible than adults topredation andstarvation; they gain experience in feeding and avoiding predators during this time.[57] They learn social and fighting skills, often through playing.[57] Primates, especially females, have longer lifespans than other similarly sized mammals,[57] this may be partially due to their slower metabolisms.[127] Late in life, female catarrhine primates appear to undergo a cessation of reproductive function known asmenopause; other groups are less studied.[128]

Diet and feeding

Leaf eatingmantled guereza
Amouse lemur eating fruit

Primates exploit a variety of food sources. It has been said that many characteristics of modern primates, including humans, derive from an early ancestor's practice of taking most of its food from the tropical canopy.[129] Most primates include fruit in their diets to obtain easily digested nutrients includingcarbohydrates andlipids for energy.[57] Primates in the suborderStrepsirrhini (non-tarsier prosimians) are able to synthesizevitamin C, like most other mammals, while primates of the suborderHaplorhini (tarsiers, monkeys and apes) have lost this ability, and require the vitamin in their diet.[130]

Many primates have anatomical specializations that enable them to exploit particular foods, such as fruit, leaves, gum orinsects.[57] For example, leaf eaters such as howler monkeys,black-and-white colobuses andsportive lemurs have extended digestive tracts which enable them to absorb nutrients from leaves that can be difficult to digest.[57]Marmosets, which are gum eaters, have strongincisor teeth, enabling them to open tree bark to get to the gum, and claws rather than nails, enabling them to cling to trees while feeding.[57] Theaye-aye combines rodent-like teeth with a long, thin middle finger to fill the same ecological niche as a woodpecker. It taps on trees to find insect larvae, then gnaws holes in the wood and inserts its elongated middle finger to pull the larvae out.[131] Some species have additional specializations. For example, thegrey-cheeked mangabey has thickenamel on its teeth, enabling it to open hard fruits and seeds that other monkeys cannot.[57] Thegelada is the only primate species that feeds primarily on grass.[132]

Hunting

Portrait of a Dayak hunter in Borneo with a boar over his shoulder
Humans have traditionally hunted prey for subsistence.

Tarsiers are the onlyextantobligate carnivorous primates, exclusively eating insects, crustaceans, small vertebrates and snakes (includingvenomous species).[133]Capuchin monkeys can exploit many different types of plant matter, including fruit, leaves, flowers, buds, nectar and seeds, but also eat insects and otherinvertebrates, bird eggs, and small vertebrates such as birds,lizards,squirrels andbats.[92]

Thecommon chimpanzee eats anomnivorousfrugivorous diet. It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant. It also eats leaves and leaf buds, seeds, blossoms, stems, pith, bark and resin. Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet, estimated as 2%.[134][135] The meat consumption includes predation on other primate species, such as thewestern red colobus monkey.[117] Thebonobo is anomnivorousfrugivore – the majority of its diet is fruit, but it supplements this with leaves, meat from smallvertebrates, such asanomalures,flying squirrels andduikers,[136] andinvertebrates.[137] In some instances, bonobos have been shown to consume lower-order primates.[138][139]

Until the development of agriculture approximately 10,000 years ago,Homo sapiens employed a hunter-gatherer method as their sole means of food collection. This involved combining stationary food sources (such as fruits, grains, tubers, and mushrooms, insect larvae and aquatic mollusks) withwild game, which must be hunted and killed in order to be consumed.[140] It has been proposed that humans have used fire to prepare andcook food since the time ofHomo erectus.[141] Around ten thousand years ago,humans developed agriculture,[142] which substantially altered their diet. This change in diet may also have altered human biology; with the spread ofdairy farming providing a new and rich source of food, leading to the evolution of the ability to digestlactose in some adults.[143][144]

As prey

Predators of primates include various species ofcarnivorans,birds of prey,reptiles, and other primates. Even gorillas have been recorded as prey. Predators of primates have diverse hunting strategies and as such, primates have evolved several differentantipredator adaptations includingcrypsis,alarm calls andmobbing. Several species have separate alarm calls for different predators such as air-borne or ground-dwelling predators. Predation may have shaped group size in primates as species exposed to higher predation pressures appear to live in larger groups.[145]

Communication

Further information:Great ape language

Problems playing these files? Seemedia help.
A pair ofblack howler monkeys vocalizing

Lemurs,lorises,tarsiers, and New World monkeys rely onolfactory signals for many aspects of social and reproductive behavior.[71] Specialized glands are used tomark territories withpheromones, which are detected by thevomeronasal organ; this process forms a large part of the communication behavior of these primates.[71] In Old World monkeys and apes this ability is mostlyvestigial, having regressed astrichromatic eyes evolved to become the main sensory organ.[146] Primates also use vocalizations, gestures, and facial expressions to convey psychological state.[147][148] Facial musculature is very developed in primates, particularly in monkeys and apes, allowing for complex facial communication. Like humans, chimpanzees can distinguish the faces of familiar and unfamiliar individuals.[149] Hand and arm gestures are also important forms of communication for great apes and a single gesture can have multiple functions.[148] Chest-beating in male gorillas is a form of visual and non-vocal sound communication that serves to show fitness to both rivals and females.[150]

Primates are a particularly vocal group of mammals.[81]Indris andblack-and-white ruffed lemurs make distinctive, loud songs and choruses which maintain territories and act asalarm calls.[151] ThePhilippine tarsier, has a high-frequency limit of auditory sensitivity of approximately 91 kHz with a dominant frequency of 70 kHz, among the highest recorded for any terrestrial mammal. For Philippine tarsiers, these ultrasonic vocalizations might represent a private channel of communication that subverts detection by predators, prey and competitors, enhances energetic efficiency, or improves detection against low-frequency background noise.[152] Malehowler monkeys are among the loudest land mammals as their roars can be heard up to 4.8 km (3.0 mi), and relate to intergroup spacing, territorial protection and possibly mate-guarding.[153][154] Male and femalesiamangs both possess inflatable pouches in the throat with which pair -bonds use to sing "duets" to each other.[155] Thevervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of at least four different predators, and the reactions of other monkeys vary according to the call.[156] Furthermore, many primate species includingchimpanzees,[157]Campbell's mona monkeys[158] orDiana monkeys[159] have been shown to combine vocalizations in sequences, suggestingsyntax may not be uniquely humans as previously thought but rather evolutionary ancient, and its origins may be deeply rooted in the primate lineage.[160]

Consonant- and vowel-like sounds exist in some orangutan calls and they maintain their meaning over great distances.[161] The time range for the evolution of human language and/or its anatomical prerequisites extends, at least in principle, from the phylogenetic divergence ofHomo (2.3 to 2.4 million years ago) fromPan (5 to 6 million years ago) to the emergence of fullbehavioral modernity some 50,000–150,000 years ago. Few dispute thatAustralopithecus probably lacked vocal communication significantly more sophisticated than that ofgreat apes in general.[162]

Intelligence and cognition

Main article:Primate cognition

Primates have advanced cognitive abilities: some make tools and use them to acquire food and for social displays;[163][164] some can perform tasks requiring cooperation, influence and rank;[165] they are status conscious, manipulative and capable of deception;[166][167] they can recognisekin andconspecifics;[168][169] and they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax and concepts of number and numerical sequence.[170][171][172] Research in primate cognition explores problem solving, memory, social interaction, atheory of mind, and numerical, spatial, and abstract concepts.[173] Comparative studies show a trend towards higher intelligence going from prosimians to New World monkeys to Old World monkeys, and significantly higher average cognitive abilities in the great apes.[174][175] However, there is a great deal of variation in each group (e.g., among New World monkeys, bothspider[174] andcapuchin monkeys[175] have scored highly by some measures), as well as in the results of different studies.[174][175]

Tool use and manufacture

Main article:Tool use by animals
Chimpanzees using twigs to dip for ants
Crab-eating macaques withstone tools

In 1960,Jane Goodall observed achimpanzee poking pieces of grass into atermite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to "fish" or "dip" for termites.[176] There are more limited reports of the closely relatedbonobo using tools in the wild; it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity.[177] It has been reported that females, both chimpanzee and bonobo, use tools more avidly than males.[178]Orangutans inBorneo scoop catfish out of small ponds. Over two years, anthropologist Anne Russon observed orangutans learning to jab sticks at catfish to scare them out of the ponds and in to their waiting hands.[179] There are few reports ofgorillas using tools in the wild. An adult femalewestern lowland gorilla used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her in crossing a pool of water. Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering, and another used a log as a bridge.[180]

The first direct observation of a non-ape primate using a tool in a wild environment occurred in 1988. Primatologist Sue Boinski watched an adult male white-faced capuchin beat afer-de-lance snake to death with a dead branch.[181] The black-striped capuchin was the first non-ape primate for which routine tool use was documented in the wild; individuals were observedcracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone.[182] In Thailand and Myanmar,crab-eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts, oysters and other bivalves, and various types of sea snails.[183] Chacma baboons use stones as weapons; stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat to when they are threatened. Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side whereupon they tumble down the side of the cliff or fall directly to the canyon floor.[184]

Although they have not been observed to use tools in the wild, lemurs in controlled settings have been shown to be capable of understanding the functional properties of the objects they had been trained to use as tools, performing as well as tool-using haplorhines.[185]

Soon after her initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.[176] Chimpanzees have also been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water.[186] Sumatran orangutans have been observed making and using tools. They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long, snap off the twigs, fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites.[187][188] In the wild, mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill atChester Zoo (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.[189] Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools.[190]

Ecology

See also:List of primates by population
Rhesus macaque atAgra Fort,India

Humans are the mostadaptable primate species, despite having a low or narrow tolerance for many of the earth's extreme environments.[191] Currently the species is present in all eightbiogeographical realms, although their presence in theAntarctic realm is very limited toresearch stations and annually there is a population decline in the winter months of this realm.

Non-human primates primarily live in thetropical latitudes of Africa, Asia, and the Americas. Species that live outside of the tropics include theJapanese macaque which lives in the Japanese islands ofHonshū andHokkaido; theBarbary macaque which lives in North Africa and several species of langur which live in China. Primates tend to live intropical rainforests but are also found intemperate forests,savannas,deserts, mountains and coastal areas.[192] The number of primate species within tropical areas has been shown to be positivelycorrelated to the amount of rainfall and the amount of rain forest area.[193] Accounting for 25% to 40% of thefruit-eating animals (byweight) within tropical rainforests, primates play an importantecological role by dispersing seeds of many tree species.[194]

Primate habitats span a range of altitudes: theblack snub-nosed monkey has been found living in theHengduan Mountains at altitudes of 4,700 meters (15,400 ft),[195] themountain gorilla can be found at 4,200 meters (13,200 ft) crossing theVirunga Mountains,[196] and the gelada has been found at elevations of up to 5,000 m (16,000 ft) in theEthiopian Highlands.[197] Some species interact with aquatic environments and may swim or even dive, including theproboscis monkey,De Brazza's monkey andAllen's swamp monkey.[198] Some primates, such as therhesus macaque and gray langurs, can exploit human-modified environments and evenlive in cities.[110][199]

Interactions between humans and other primates

Disease transmission

Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create pathways for the transmission ofzoonotic diseases. Viruses such asHerpesviridae (most notablyHerpes B Virus),Poxviridae,measles,ebola,rabies, theMarburg virus andviral hepatitis can be transmitted to humans; in some cases the viruses produce potentially fatal diseases in both humans and non-human primates.[200]

Legal and social status

Further information:Great ape personhood
Slow lorises are popular in theexotic pet trade, which threatens wild populations.

Onlyhumans are recognized as persons and protected in law by theUnited NationsUniversal Declaration of Human Rights.[b] The legal status of NHPs, on the other hand, is the subject of much debate, with organizations such as theGreat Ape Project (GAP) campaigning toaward at least some of them legal rights.[202] In June 2008, Spain became the first country in the world to recognize the rights of some NHPs, when its parliament's cross-party environmental committee urged the country to comply with GAP's recommendations, which are thatchimpanzees,orangutans andgorillas are not to be used for animal experiments.[203][204]

Many species of NHP are kept as pets by humans. The Allied Effort to Save Other Primates (AESOP) estimates that around 15,000 NHPs live as exotic pets in the United States.[205] The expanding Chinese middle class has increased demand for NHPs as exotic pets in recent years.[206] Although NHP import for the pet trade was banned in the U.S. in 1975, smuggling still occurs along theUnited States – Mexico border, with prices ranging fromUS$3000 for monkeys to $30,000 for apes.[207]

Primates are used asmodel organisms in laboratories and have been used inspace missions.[208] They serve asservice animals for disabled humans.Capuchin monkeys can betrained to assistquadriplegic humans; their intelligence, memory, and manual dexterity make them ideal helpers.[209]

NHPs are kept inzoos around the globe. Historically, zoos were primarily a form of entertainment, but more recently have shifted their focus towards conservation, education and research. GAP does not insist that all NHPs should be released from zoos, primarily because captive-born primates lack the knowledge and experience to survive in the wild if released.[210]

Role in scientific research

Further information:Animal testing on non-human primates andInternational trade in primates
Sam, arhesus macaque, was flown to the edge of space byNASA in the 1959Little Joe 2 flight ofProject Mercury.

Thousands of non-human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans.[211][212] In particular, the brains and eyes of NHPs more closely parallel human anatomy than those of any other animals. NHPs are commonly used inpreclinical trials,neuroscience,ophthalmology studies, and toxicity studies.Rhesus macaques are often used, as are othermacaques, Africangreen monkeys, chimpanzees,baboons,squirrel monkeys, andmarmosets, both wild-caught and purpose-bred.[211][213]

In 2005, GAP reported that 1,280 of the 3,100 NHPs living in captivity in the United States were used for experiments.[202] In 2004, theEuropean Union used around 10,000 NHPs in such experiments; in 2005 in Great Britain, 4,652 experiments were conducted on 3,115 NHPs.[214] Governments of many nations have strict care requirements of NHPs kept in captivity. In the US, federal guidelines extensively regulate aspects of NHP housing, feeding, enrichment, and breeding.[215] European groups such as theEuropean Coalition to End Animal Experiments are seeking a ban on all NHP use in experiments as part of the European Union's review of animal testing legislation.[216]

Extinction threats

Humans are known to hunt other primates for food, calledbushmeat. Pictured are two men who have killed a number ofsilky sifaka andwhite-headed brown lemurs.

TheInternational Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) lists more than a third of primates as critically endangered or vulnerable. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction, including: 87% of species in Madagascar, 73% in Asia, 37% in Africa, and 36% in South and Central America.[217] Additionally, 75% of primate species have decreasing populations.[217] Trade is regulated, as all species are listed byCITES inAppendix II, except 50 species and subspecies listed inAppendix I, which gain full protection from trade.[218][219]

Common threats to primate species includedeforestation,forest fragmentation,monkey drives (resulting from primate crop raiding),[220] and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing is widely regarded as the process that most threatens primates.[221][222][223] More than 90% of primate species occur in tropical forests.[222][224] The main cause of forest loss is clearing for agriculture, although commercial logging,subsistence harvesting of timber, mining, and dam construction also contribute to tropical forest destruction.[224] In Indonesia large areas of lowland forest have been cleared to increasepalm oil production, and one analysis of satellite imagery concluded that during 1998 and 1999 there was a loss of 1,000Sumatran orangutans per year in theLeuser Ecosystem alone.[225]

The critically endangeredsilky sifaka

Primates with a large body size (over 5 kg) are at increased extinction risk due to their greater profitability topoachers compared to smaller primates.[224] They reach sexual maturity later and have a longer period between births. Populations therefore recover more slowly after being depleted by poaching or the pet trade.[226] Data for some African cities show that half of all protein consumed in urban areas comes from thebushmeat trade.[227] Endangered primates such asguenons and thedrill are hunted at levels that far exceed sustainable levels.[227] This is due to their large body size, ease of transport and profitability per animal.[227] As farming encroaches on forest habitats, primates feed on the crops, causing the farmers large economic losses.[228] Primate crop raiding gives locals a negative impression of primates, hindering conservation efforts.[229]

Madagascar, home to five endemic primate families, has experienced the greatest extinction of the recent past; since human settlement 1,500 years ago, at least eight classes and fifteen of the larger species have become extinct due to hunting and habitat destruction.[71] Among the primates wiped out wereArchaeoindris (a lemur larger than a silverback gorilla) and the familiesPalaeopropithecidae andArchaeolemuridae.[71]

Thecritically endangered Sumatran orangutan

In Asia,Hinduism,Buddhism, andIslam prohibit eating primate meat; however, primates are still hunted for food.[224] Some smaller traditional religions allow the consumption of primate meat.[230][231] The pet trade and traditional medicine also increase demand for illegal hunting.[206][232][233] Therhesus macaque, amodel organism, was protected after excessive trapping threatened its numbers in the 1960s; the program was so effective that they are now viewed as a pest throughout their range.[223]

In Central and South America, forest fragmentation and hunting are the two main problems for primates. Large tracts of forest are now rare in Central America.[221][234] This increases the amount of forest vulnerable toedge effects such as farmland encroachment, lower levels of humidity and a change in plant life.[235][236] Movement restriction results in a greater amount of inbreeding, which can cause deleterious effects leading to apopulation bottleneck, whereby a significant percentage of the population is lost.[237][238]

There are 21 critically endangered primates, seven of which have remained on the IUCN's "The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates" list since the year 2000: thesilky sifaka,Delacour's langur, thewhite-headed langur, thegray-shanked douc, theTonkin snub-nosed monkey, theCross River gorilla and theSumatran orangutan.[239]Miss Waldron's red colobus was recently declared extinct when no trace of the subspecies could be found from 1993 to 1999.[240] A few hunters have found and killed individuals since then, but the subspecies' prospects remain bleak.[241]

See also

Footnotes

  1. ^Although themonophyletic relationship between lemurs and lorisoids is widely accepted, their clade name is not. The term "lemuriform" is used here because it derives from one popular taxonomy that clumps theclade of toothcombed primates into oneinfraorder and the extinct, non-toothcombedadapiforms into another, both within thesuborder Strepsirrhini.[20][21] However, another popular alternative taxonomy places thelorisoids in their own infraorder, Lorisiformes.[22]
  2. ^Article 6: Everyone has the right to recognition everywhere as a person before the law.[201]

References

  1. ^abcdefgGroves, C. P. (2005).Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.).Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 111–184.ISBN 0-801-88221-4.OCLC 62265494.
  2. ^Silcox, Mary T.; Bloch, Jonathan I.; Boyer, Doug M.; Chester, Stephen G. B.; López-Torres, Sergi (2017)."The evolutionary radiation of plesiadapiforms".Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews.26 (2):74–94.doi:10.1002/evan.21526.ISSN 1520-6505.PMID 28429568.
  3. ^"Primate".Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary.Merriam-Webster. Retrieved2008-07-21.
  4. ^The Book of Popular Science. 1963. p. 257.
  5. ^Chisholm, Hugh, ed. (1911)."Ape" .Encyclopædia Britannica. Vol. 02 (11th ed.). Cambridge University Press. p. 160.
  6. ^abWeisberger, Mindy (March 23, 2024)."Why don't humans have tails? Scientists find answers in an unlikely place".CNN.Archived from the original on March 24, 2024. RetrievedMarch 24, 2024.
  7. ^Dixson, A.F. (1981),The Natural History of the Gorilla, London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson,ISBN 978-0-297-77895-0
  8. ^Definitions ofparaphyly vary; for the one used here see e.g.Stace, Clive A. (2010),"Classification by molecules: What's in it for field botanists?"(PDF),Watsonia,28:103–122, archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2011-07-26, retrieved2010-02-07.
  9. ^Definitions ofmonophyly vary; for the one used here see e.g.Mishler, Brent D (2009), "Species are not Uniquely Real Biological Entities", in Ayala, F.J. & Arp, R. (eds.),Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Biology, pp. 110–122,doi:10.1002/9781444314922.ch6,ISBN 978-1-4443-1492-2.
  10. ^Benton 2005, p. 371.
  11. ^Benton 2005, pp. 378–380.
  12. ^Linnaeus, C. (1758).Sistema naturae per regna tria Naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus differentiis, synonimis locis. Tomus I. Impensis direct. Laurentii Salvii, Holmia. pp. 20–32.
  13. ^Linnaeus, C. (1735).Sistema naturae sive regna tria Naturae systematice proposita per classes, ordines, genera, & species. apud Theodorum Haak, Lugduni Batavorum. pp. s.p.
  14. ^Blainville, H. (1839). "Nouvelle classification des Mammifères".Annales Françaises et Etrangères d'Anatomie et de Physiologie Appliquées à la Médicine et à l'Histoire Naturelle, 3. pp. 268–269.
  15. ^Thorington, R. W. & Anderson, S. (1984)."Primates". In Anderson, S. & Jones, J. K. (eds.).Orders and Families of Recent Mammals of the World. New York: John Wiley and Sons. pp. 187–217.ISBN 978-0-471-08493-8.
  16. ^McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997).Classification of Mammals: Above the species level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 631.ISBN 0-231-11013-8.
  17. ^Strier, K. (2007).Primate Behavioral Ecology (Third ed.). Pearson Allyn and Bacon. pp. 50–53.ISBN 978-0-205-44432-8.
  18. ^Cartmill, M.; Smith, F. H. (2011).The Human Lineage. John Wiley & Sons.ISBN 978-1-118-21145-8.
  19. ^abGroves, C. P. (2001).Primate Taxonomy. Smithsonian Institution Press.ISBN 1-56098-872-X.
  20. ^Szalay & Delson 1980, p. 149.
  21. ^Cartmill 2010, p. 15.
  22. ^Hartwig 2011, pp. 20–21.
  23. ^Janečka, J. E.; Miller, W.; Pringle, T. H.; Wiens, F.; Zitzmann, A.; Helgen, K. M.; Springer, M. S.; Murphy, W. J. (2 November 2007). "Molecular and Genomic Data Identify the Closest Living Relative of Primates".Science.318 (5851):792–794.Bibcode:2007Sci...318..792J.doi:10.1126/science.1147555.PMID 17975064.S2CID 12251814.
  24. ^Kavanagh, M. (1983).A Complete Guide to Monkeys, Apes and Other Primates. New York: Viking Press. pp. 18.ISBN 0-670-43543-0.
  25. ^McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997).Classification of Mammals Above the Species Level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 329.ISBN 0-231-11012-X.
  26. ^Williams, B.A.; Kay, R.F.; Kirk, E.C. (2010)."New perspectives on anthropoid origins".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.107 (11):4797–4804.Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W.doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107.PMC 2841917.PMID 20212104.
  27. ^Stanyon, Roscoe; Springer, Mark S.; Meredith, Robert W.; Gatesy, John; Emerling, Christopher A.; Park, Jong; Rabosky, Daniel L.; Stadler, Tanja; Steiner, Cynthia; Ryder, Oliver A.; Janečka, Jan E.; Fisher, Colleen A.; Murphy, William J. (2012)."Macroevolutionary Dynamics and Historical Biogeography of Primate Diversification Inferred from a Species Supermatrix".PLOS ONE.7 (11): e49521.Bibcode:2012PLoSO...749521S.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049521.ISSN 1932-6203.PMC 3500307.PMID 23166696.
  28. ^Jameson, Natalie M.; Hou, Zhuo-Cheng; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Weckle, Amy; Goodman, Morris; Steiper, Michael E.; Wildman, Derek E. (September 2011). "Genomic data reject the hypothesis of a prosimian primate clade".Journal of Human Evolution.61 (3):295–305.Bibcode:2011JHumE..61..295J.doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.04.004.ISSN 0047-2484.PMID 21620437.
  29. ^Pozzi, Luca; Hodgson, Jason A.; Burrell, Andrew S.; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Raaum, Ryan L.; Disotell, Todd R. (June 2014)."Primate phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes".Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution.75:165–183.Bibcode:2014MolPE..75..165P.doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2014.02.023.ISSN 1055-7903.PMC 4059600.PMID 24583291.
  30. ^Stanyon, Roscoe; Finstermeier, Knut; Zinner, Dietmar; Brameier, Markus; Meyer, Matthias; Kreuz, Eva; Hofreiter, Michael; Roos, Christian (16 July 2013)."A Mitogenomic Phylogeny of Living Primates".PLOS ONE.8 (7): e69504.Bibcode:2013PLoSO...869504F.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069504.ISSN 1932-6203.PMC 3713065.PMID 23874967.
  31. ^abO'Leary, M. A.; et al. (8 February 2013). "The placental mammal ancestor and the post–K-Pg radiation of placentals".Science.339 (6120):662–667.Bibcode:2013Sci...339..662O.doi:10.1126/science.1229237.hdl:11336/7302.PMID 23393258.S2CID 206544776.
  32. ^Wilson Mantilla, Gregory P.; Chester, Stephen G. B.; Clemens, William A.; Moore, Jason R.; Sprain, Courtney J.; Hovatter, Brody T.; Mitchell, William S.; Mans, Wade W.; Mundil, Roland; Renne, Paul R. (2021)."Earliest Palaeocene purgatoriids and the initial radiation of stem primates".Royal Society Open Science.8 (2): 210050.Bibcode:2021RSOS....810050W.doi:10.1098/rsos.210050.PMC 8074693.PMID 33972886.
  33. ^Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Kirk, E. C. (2010)."New perspectives on anthropoid origins".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.107 (11):4797–4804.Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W.doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107.PMC 2841917.PMID 20212104.
  34. ^Miller, E. R.; Gunnell, G. F.; Martin, R. D. (2005)."Deep Time and the Search for Anthropoid Origins"(PDF).American Journal of Physical Anthropology.128:60–95.doi:10.1002/ajpa.20352.PMID 16369958.
  35. ^abChatterjee, Helen J; Ho, Simon Y.W.; Barnes, Ian;Groves, Colin (27 October 2009)."Estimating the phylogeny and divergence times of primates using a supermatrix approach".BMC Evolutionary Biology.9 (1): 259.Bibcode:2009BMCEE...9..259C.doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-259.PMC 2774700.PMID 19860891.
  36. ^Lee, M. (September 1999)."Molecular Clock Calibrations and Metazoan Divergence Dates".Journal of Molecular Evolution.49 (3):385–391.Bibcode:1999JMolE..49..385L.doi:10.1007/PL00006562.PMID 10473780.S2CID 1629316.
  37. ^"Scientists Push Back Primate Origins From 65 Million To 85 Million Years Ago".Science Daily. Retrieved2008-10-24.
  38. ^Tavaré, S.; Marshall, C. R.; Will, O.; Soligo, C.; Martin R.D. (April 18, 2002)."Using the fossil record to estimate the age of the last common ancestor of extant primates".Nature.416 (6882):726–729.Bibcode:2002Natur.416..726T.doi:10.1038/416726a.PMID 11961552.S2CID 4368374.
  39. ^Klonisch, T.; Froehlich, C.; Tetens, F.; Fischer, B.; Hombach-Klonisch, S. (2001)."Molecular Remodeling of Members of the Relaxin Family During Primate Evolution".Molecular Biology and Evolution.18 (3):393–403.doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a003815.PMID 11230540.
  40. ^abcdefHorvath, J.; et al. (2008)."Development and Application of a Phylogenomic Toolkit: Resolving the Evolutionary History of Madagascar's Lemurs".Genome Research.18 (3):489–499.doi:10.1101/gr.7265208.PMC 2259113.PMID 18245770.
  41. ^Mittermeier, R.; Ganzhorn, J.; Konstant, W.; Glander, K.; Tattersall, I.; Groves, C.; Rylands, A.; Hapke, A.; Ratsimbazafy, J.; Mayor, M.; Louis, E.; Rumpler, Y.; Schwitzer, C.; Rasoloarison, R. (December 2008)."Lemur Diversity in Madagascar".International Journal of Primatology.29 (6):1607–1656.doi:10.1007/s10764-008-9317-y.S2CID 17614597. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2021-02-15. Retrieved2019-09-24.
  42. ^abcdSellers, Bill (2000-10-20)."Primate Evolution"(PDF). University of Edinburgh. pp. 13–17. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2008-10-29. Retrieved2008-10-23.
  43. ^abcdHartwig, W. (2007). "Primate Evolution". In Campbell, C.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. (eds.).Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–17.ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  44. ^Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Christopher Kirk, E.; Ross, C. F. (2010)."Darwinius masillae is a strepsirrhine—a reply to Franzenet al. (2009)"(PDF).Journal of Human Evolution.59 (5):567–573, discussion 573–9.Bibcode:2010JHumE..59..567W.doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.01.003.PMID 20188396. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2013-05-17. Retrieved2015-09-04.
  45. ^Ciochon, R. & Fleagle, J. (1987).Primate Evolution and Human Origins. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. p. 72.ISBN 978-0-202-01175-2.
  46. ^abGarbutt, N. (2007).Mammals of Madagascar, A Complete Guide. A&C Black Publishers. pp. 85–86.ISBN 978-0-300-12550-4.
  47. ^abMittermeier, R.A.; et al. (2006).Lemurs of Madagascar (2nd ed.). Conservation International. pp. 23–26.ISBN 1-881173-88-7.
  48. ^Shekelle, M. (2005).Evolutionary Biology of Tarsiers. Archived fromthe original on 2008-09-07. Retrieved2008-08-22.
  49. ^Schmidt, T.; et al. (3 May 2005)."Rapid electrostatic evolution at the binding site for cytochrome c on cytochrome c oxidase in anthropoid primates".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.102 (18):6379–6384.Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.6379S.doi:10.1073/pnas.0409714102.PMC 1088365.PMID 15851671.
  50. ^Wade, Lizzie (June 5, 2013)."Early Primate Weighed Less Than an Ounce".ScienceNow. Archived fromthe original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved2013-06-07.
  51. ^Kay, R. F. (2012)."Evidence for an Asian origin of stem anthropoid s".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.109 (26):10132–10133.Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910132K.doi:10.1073/pnas.1207933109.PMC 3387095.PMID 22699505.
  52. ^Chaimanee, Y.; Chavasseau, O.; Beard, K. C.; Kyaw, A. A.; Soe, A. N.; Sein, C.; Lazzari, V.; Marivaux, L.; Marandat, B.; Swe, M.; Rugbumrung, M.; Lwin, T.; Valentin, X.; Zin-Maung-Maung-Thein; Jaeger, J. -J. (2012)."Late Middle Eocene primate from Myanmar and the initial anthropoid colonization of Africa".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.109 (26):10293–10297.Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910293C.doi:10.1073/pnas.1200644109.PMC 3387043.PMID 22665790.
  53. ^Marivaux, L.; et al. (2005-06-14)."Anthropoid primates from the Oligocene of Pakistan (Bugti Hills): Data on early anthropoid evolution and biogeography".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.102 (24):8436–8441.Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8436M.doi:10.1073/pnas.0503469102.PMC 1150860.PMID 15937103.
  54. ^Schrago, C.G. & Russo, C.A.M. (2003)."Timing the Origin of New World Monkeys"(PDF Reprint).Molecular Biology and Evolution.20 (10):1620–1625.doi:10.1093/molbev/msg172.PMID 12832653.
  55. ^Houle, A. (1999)."The origin of platyrrhines: An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island model".American Journal of Physical Anthropology.109 (4):541–559.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199908)109:4<541::AID-AJPA9>3.0.CO;2-N.PMID 10423268.
  56. ^Andrews, P. & Kelley, J. (2007). "Middle Miocene Dispersals of Apes".Folia Primatologica.78 (5–6):328–343.doi:10.1159/000105148.PMID 17855786.S2CID 19293586.
  57. ^abcdefghijklmnopStrier, K. (2007).Primate Behavioral Ecology (3rd ed.).Allyn & Bacon. pp. 7, 64, 71, 77,182–185,273–280, 284,287–298.ISBN 978-0-205-44432-8.
  58. ^Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Evolution and the Emergence of Humans".Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 650.ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  59. ^IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (1 March 2021)."Primate diversity by region". International Union for the Conservation of Nature.
  60. ^Tenaza, R. (1984). "Songs of hybrid gibbons (Hylobates lar ×H. muelleri)".American Journal of Primatology.8 (3):249–253.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350080307.PMID 31986810.S2CID 84957700.
  61. ^abBernsteil, I. S. (1966). "Naturally occurring primate hybrid".Science.154 (3756):1559–1560.Bibcode:1966Sci...154.1559B.doi:10.1126/science.154.3756.1559.PMID 4958933.S2CID 85898043.
  62. ^Sugawara, K. (January 1979). "Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid baboons betweenPapio anubis andP. hamadryas in the Awash Valley, Ethiopia".Primates.20 (1):21–56.doi:10.1007/BF02373827.S2CID 23061688.
  63. ^Jolly, C. J.; Woolley-Barker, Tamsin; et al. (1997). "Intergeneric Hybrid Baboons".International Journal of Primatology.18 (4):597–627.doi:10.1023/A:1026367307470.S2CID 27900830.
  64. ^Liu, Zhen; et al. (24 January 2018)."Cloning of Macaque Monkeys by Somatic Cell Nuclear Transfer".Cell.172 (4): 881–887.e7.doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.01.020.PMID 29395327.
  65. ^Normile, Dennis (24 January 2018)."These monkey twins are the first primate clones made by the method that developed Dolly".Science.doi:10.1126/science.aat1066. Retrieved24 January 2018.
  66. ^Cyranoski, David (24 January 2018)."First monkeys cloned with technique that made Dolly the sheep - Chinese scientists create cloned primates that could revolutionize studies of human disease".Nature.553 (7689):387–388.Bibcode:2018Natur.553..387C.doi:10.1038/d41586-018-01027-z.PMID 29368720.
  67. ^Briggs, Helen (24 January 2018)."First monkey clones created in Chinese laboratory".BBC News. Retrieved24 January 2018.
  68. ^"Scientists Successfully Clone Monkeys; Are Humans Up Next?".The New York Times. Associated Press. 24 January 2018. Retrieved24 January 2018.
  69. ^abcdPough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Characteristics of Primates".Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 630.ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  70. ^abAiello, L. & Dean, C. (1990).An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy. Academic Press. pp. 193.ISBN 0-12-045590-0.
  71. ^abcdef"Primate".Encyclopædia Britannica Online.Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc. 2008. Retrieved2008-07-21.
  72. ^abcdefMyers, P. (1999).""Primates" (On-line)". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved2008-06-03.
  73. ^Caves, Eleanor M. (May 2018)."Visual Acuity and the Evolution of Signals".Trends in Ecology & Evolution.33 (5):358–372.Bibcode:2018TEcoE..33..358C.doi:10.1016/j.tree.2018.03.001.PMID 29609907. Retrieved29 July 2018.
  74. ^Kirk, E. Christopher; Kay, Richard F. (2004), Ross, Callum F.; Kay, Richard F. (eds.),"The Evolution of High Visual Acuity in the Anthropoidea",Anthropoid Origins: New Visions, Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects, Boston, MA: Springer US, pp. 539–602,doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-8873-7_20,ISBN 978-1-4419-8873-7, retrieved2023-07-30
  75. ^Campbell, B. G. & Loy, J. D. (2000).Humankind Emerging (8th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. p. 85.ISBN 0-673-52364-0.
  76. ^abWhite, T. & Kazlev, A. (2006-01-08)."Archonta: Primates".Palaeos. Archived fromthe original on 2008-05-12. Retrieved2008-06-03.
  77. ^abcdefMacdonald, David (2006). "Primates".The Encyclopedia of Mammals. The Brown Reference Group plc. pp. 282–307.ISBN 0-681-45659-0.
  78. ^Ash, M. M.; Nelson, S. J.; Wheeler, R. C. (2003).Wheeler's Dental Anatomy, Physiology, and Occlusion. W.B. Saunders. p. 12.ISBN 978-0-7216-9382-8.
  79. ^Garber PA, Rehg JA (November 1999)."The ecological role of the prehensile tail in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus)".American Journal of Physical Anthropology.110 (3):325–39.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199911)110:3<325::AID-AJPA5>3.0.CO;2-D.PMID 10516564.
  80. ^Russo GA, Young JW (November 2011)."Tail growth tracks the ontogeny of prehensile tail use in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and C. apella)".American Journal of Physical Anthropology.146 (3):465–73.doi:10.1002/ajpa.21617.PMID 21953012.
  81. ^abFriderun Ankel-Simons (27 July 2010).Primate Anatomy: An Introduction. Academic Press. pp. 442, 521.ISBN 978-0-08-046911-9.
  82. ^Ralls, K. (1976). "Mammals in Which Females are Larger Than Males".The Quarterly Review of Biology.51 (2):245–76.doi:10.1086/409310.PMID 785524.S2CID 25927323.
  83. ^Lindstedtand & Boyce; Boyce, Mark S. (July 1985). "Seasonality, Fasting Endurance, and Body Size in Mammals".The American Naturalist.125 (6): 873.Bibcode:1985ANat..125..873L.doi:10.1086/284385.S2CID 84308684.
  84. ^Frisch, J. E. (1963). "Sex-differences in the canines of the gibbon (Hylobates lar)".Primates.4 (2):1–10.doi:10.1007/BF01659148.S2CID 189798134.
  85. ^Kay, R. F. (1975). "The functional adaptations of primate molar teeth".American Journal of Physical Anthropology.43 (2):195–215.doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430207.PMID 810034.
  86. ^Crook, J. H. (1972)."Sexual selection, dimorphism, and social organization in the primates". In Campbell, B. G. (ed.).Sexual selection and the descent of man. Aldine Transaction. pp. 246.ISBN 978-0-202-02005-1.
  87. ^abCheverud, J. M.; Dow, M. M.; Leutenegger, W. (November 1985)."The quantitative assessment of phylogenetic constraints in comparative analyses: Sexual dimorphism in body weight among primates".Evolution.39 (6):1335–1351.doi:10.2307/2408790.JSTOR 2408790.PMID 28564267.
  88. ^Leutenegger, W.; Cheverud, J. M. (1982)."Correlates of sexual dimorphism in primates: Ecological and size variables".International Journal of Primatology.3 (4):387–402.doi:10.1007/BF02693740.S2CID 38220186.
  89. ^abPlavcan, J. M. (2001)."Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution".American Journal of Physical Anthropology.33:25–53.doi:10.1002/ajpa.10011.PMID 11786990.S2CID 31722173.
  90. ^O'Higgins, P.; Collard, M. (2002). "Sexual dimorphism and facial growth in papionine monkeys".Journal of Zoology.257 (2):255–72.doi:10.1017/S0952836902000857.
  91. ^Sussman, R. W. (1999).Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 1: Lorises, Lemurs and Tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 78,89–90, 108,121–123, 233.ISBN 0-536-02256-9.
  92. ^abcdeSussman, R. W. (2003).Primate Ecology and Social Structure, Volume 2: New World Monkeys (Revised First ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 77–80,132–133,141–143.ISBN 0-536-74364-9.
  93. ^Glazier, S. D.; Flowerday, C. A. (2003).Selected Readings in the Anthropology of Religion: Theoretical and Methodological Essays. Greenwood Publishing Group. pp. 53.ISBN 9780313300905.
  94. ^Arrese, C. A.; Oddy, Alison Y.; et al. (2005)."Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)".Proceedings of the Royal Society B.272 (1565):791–6.doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009.PMC 1599861.PMID 15888411.
  95. ^Bowmaker, J. K.; Astell, S.; Hunt, D. M.; Mollon, J. D. (1991)."Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys"(PDF).The Journal of Experimental Biology.156 (1):1–19.Bibcode:1991JExpB.156....1B.doi:10.1242/jeb.156.1.1.ISSN 0022-0949.PMID 2051127. Retrieved2008-06-16.
  96. ^abSurridge, A. K. & D. Osorio (2003). "Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates".Trends in Ecology and Evolution.18 (4):198–205.doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00012-0.
  97. ^Lucas, P. W.; Dominy, N. J.; Riba-Hernandez, P.; Stoner, K. E.; Yamashita, N.; Loría-Calderón, E.; Petersen-Pereira, W.; Rojas-Durán, Y.; Salas-Pena, R.; Solis-Madrigal, S.; Osorio, D.; Darvell, B. W. (2003)."Evolution and function of routine trichromatic vision in primates".Evolution.57 (11):2636–43.doi:10.1554/03-168.PMID 14686538.S2CID 739130.
  98. ^abcdeWrangham, R. W. (1982). "Mutualism, kinship and social evolution".Current Problems in Sociobiology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 269–89.ISBN 0-521-24203-7.
  99. ^Goldberg, T. L.; Wrangham, R. W. (September 1997). "Genetic correlates of social behavior in wild chimpanzees: evidence from mitochondrial DNA".Animal Behaviour.54 (3):559–70.doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0450.PMID 9299041.S2CID 18223362.
  100. ^Bowdler, Neil (21 December 2010)."Neanderthal family found cannibalised in cave in Spain".BBC News.
  101. ^Bowdler, Neil (2 June 2011)."Ancient cave women 'left childhood homes'".BBC News. Retrieved2 June 2011.
  102. ^Copeland, Sandi R.; et al. (1 June 2011). "Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins".Nature.474 (7349):76–78.doi:10.1038/nature10149.PMID 21637256.S2CID 205225222.
  103. ^Fiore, A. D. & Campbell, C. J. (2007). "The Atelines". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.).Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 175.ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  104. ^Bartlett, T. Q. (2007). "The Hylobatidae". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. K. (eds.).Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 283.ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  105. ^abcdefgDixon, Alan F (2012).Primate Sexuality. Oxford University Press. pp. 32–61.ISBN 9780199544646.
  106. ^Watts D. P. (1996). "Comparative socio-ecology of gorillas". In McGrew W. C.; Marchant L. F.; Nishida, T. (eds.).Great Ape Societies. Cambridge (England: Cambridge Univ Press. pp. 16–28.ISBN 978-0521555364.
  107. ^abCharpentier MJ, Widdig A, Alberts SC (December 2007)."Inbreeding depression in non-human primates: a historical review of methods used and empirical data".American Journal of Primatology.69 (12):1370–86.doi:10.1002/ajp.20445.PMID 17486606.S2CID 46626761.
  108. ^Ralls K, Ballou J (1982)."Effect of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates"(PDF).International Journal of Primatology.3 (4):491–505.doi:10.1007/BF02693747.S2CID 10954608.[permanent dead link]
  109. ^Constable, J. L.; Ashley, M. V.; Goodall, J.; Pusey, A. E. (May 2001)."Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees".Molecular Ecology.10 (5):1279–300.Bibcode:2001MolEc..10.1279C.doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01262.x.PMID 11380884.S2CID 46604532.
  110. ^abcdeRowe, N. (1996).The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates. Pogonias Press. pp. 4, 139, 143, 15 185, 223.ISBN 0-9648825-0-7.
  111. ^Couzin, Iain D.; Laidre, Mark E. (August 2009)."Fission–fusion populations".Current Biology.19 (15):R633 –R635.Bibcode:2009CBio...19.R633C.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.034.ISSN 0960-9822.PMID 19674541.S2CID 13549970.
  112. ^abPough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Societies".Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 621–623.ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  113. ^Smuts, B.B., Cheney, D.L. Seyfarth, R.M., Wrangham, R.W., & Struhsaker, T.T. (Eds.) (1987).Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press for articles on the structure and function of various primate societies.
  114. ^Zimmer, Carl (16 November 2023)."Scientists Find First Evidence That Groups of Apes Cooperate - Some bonobos are challenging the notion that humans are the only primates capable of group-to-group alliances".The New York Times.Archived from the original on 16 November 2023. Retrieved17 November 2023.
  115. ^Samuni, Liran; et al. (16 November 2023)."Cooperation across social borders in bonobos".Science.382 (6672):805–809.Bibcode:2023Sci...382..805S.doi:10.1126/science.adg0844.PMID 37972165.Archived from the original on 17 November 2023. Retrieved17 November 2023.
  116. ^Shultz, S. & Thomsett, S. (2007). "Interactions between African Crowned Eagles and Their Prey Community". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K. & Noe, R. (eds.).Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. p. 181.ISBN 978-0-521-81633-5.
  117. ^abBshary, R. (2007). "Interactions between Red Colobus Monkeys and Chimpanzees". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Noe, R. (eds.).Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. pp. 155–170.ISBN 978-0-521-81633-5.
  118. ^Stanford, C. (1998).Chimpanzee and Red Colobus : the ecology of predator and prey. Harvard University Press. pp. 130–138, 233.ISBN 0-674-00722-0.
  119. ^abBoinski, S. (2000). "Social Manipulation Within and Between Troops Mediates Primate Group Movement". In Boinski, S.; Garber, P. (eds.).On the Move : how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press. pp. 447–448.ISBN 0-226-06340-2.
  120. ^Dupanloup, Isabelle; Pereira, Luisa; Bertorelle, Giorgio; Calafell, Francesc; Prata, Maria João; Amorim, Antonio; Barbujani, Guido (1 July 2003)."A Recent Shift from Polygyny to Monogamy in Humans Is Suggested by the Analysis of Worldwide Y-Chromosome Diversity".Journal of Molecular Evolution.57 (1):85–97.Bibcode:2003JMolE..57...85D.doi:10.1007/s00239-003-2458-x.ISSN 0022-2844.PMID 12962309. Retrieved13 July 2024 – via Springer Link.
  121. ^abDixon, Alan F (2012).Primate Sexuality. Oxford University Press. pp. 130–131, 135,149–156,200–202.ISBN 9780199544646.
  122. ^de Waal FB (March 1995)."Bonobo sex and society"(PDF).Scientific American.272 (3):82–8.Bibcode:1995SciAm.272c..82W.doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0395-82.PMID 7871411. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 27 January 2012. Retrieved21 December 2011.
  123. ^Opie, Christopher; Atkinson, Quentin D.; Dunbarc, Robin I. M.; Shultz, Susanne (2013)."Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.110 (33):13328–13332.Bibcode:2013PNAS..11013328O.doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110.PMC 3746880.PMID 23898180.
  124. ^De Ruiter, Jan R.;Van Hooff, Jan A. R. A. M. & Scheffrahn, Wolfgang (1994). "Social and genetic aspects of paternity in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis)".Behaviour.129 (3–4):203–24.doi:10.1163/156853994x00613.JSTOR 4535195.
  125. ^Kappeler, Peter M. (1998). "Nests, Tree Holes, and the Evolution of Primate Life Histories".American Journal of Primatology.46 (1):7–33.doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1998)46:1<7::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-#.PMID 9730211.S2CID 196589387.
  126. ^Ross, Caroline (1991). "Park or ride? Evolution of infant carrying in primates".International Journal of Primatology.22 (5). Kluwer Academic Publishing:749–771.doi:10.1023/A:1012065332758.S2CID 25301078.
  127. ^Mintz, Zoe (14 January 2014)."Humans And Primates Burn 50 Percent Fewer Calories Each Day Than Other Mammals".www.ibtimes.com. IBT Media Inc. Retrieved2014-01-14.
  128. ^Walker ML, Herndon JG; Herndon (2008)."Menopause in nonhuman primates?".Biology of Reproduction.79 (3):398–406.doi:10.1095/biolreprod.108.068536.PMC 2553520.PMID 18495681.
  129. ^Milton, K. (1993)."Diet and Primate Evolution"(PDF).Scientific American. Vol. 269, no. 2. pp. 86–93.Bibcode:1993SciAm.269b..86M.doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0893-86.PMID 8351513.
  130. ^Pollock, J. I.; Mullin, R. J. (1986)."Vitamin C biosynthesis in prosimians: Evidence for the anthropoid affinity of Tarsius".American Journal of Physical Anthropology.73 (1):65–70.doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330730106.PMID 3113259. Archived fromthe original on 2012-06-28. Retrieved2010-03-16.
  131. ^Milliken, G. W.; Ward, J. P.; Erickson, C. J. (1991). "Independent digit control in foraging by the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis)".Folia Primatologica.56 (4):219–224.doi:10.1159/000156551.PMID 1937286.
  132. ^Hiller, C. (2000)."Theropithecus gelada".Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved2008-08-08.
  133. ^Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (2003). "Introduction". In Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (eds.).Tarsiers Past, Present and Future. Rutgers University Press. p. 1.ISBN 0-8135-3236-1.
  134. ^Goodall, Jane (1986).The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.ISBN 0-674-11649-6.
  135. ^Guernsey, Paul."WHAT DO CHIMPS EAT?".All About Wildlife. Archived fromthe original on 2019-11-18. Retrieved2013-04-22.
  136. ^Ihobe H (1992). "Observations on the meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire".Primates.33 (2):247–250.doi:10.1007/BF02382754.S2CID 10063791.
  137. ^Rafert, J.; Vineberg, E.O. (1997)."Bonobo Nutrition – relation of captive diet to wild diet"(PDF).Bonobo Husbandry Manual. American Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2012-04-25.
  138. ^Surbeck, M; Fowler, A; Deimel, C; Hohmann, G (2008). "Evidence for the consumption of arboreal, diurnal primates by bonobos (Pan paniscus)".American Journal of Primatology.71 (2):171–4.doi:10.1002/ajp.20634.PMID 19058132.S2CID 32622605.
  139. ^Surbeck M, Hohmann G; Hohmann (14 October 2008)."Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park".Current Biology.18 (19): R906–7.Bibcode:2008CBio...18.R906S.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.040.PMID 18957233.S2CID 6708310.
  140. ^Cordain L, Eaton SB, Sebastian A, et al. (February 2005)."Origins and evolution of the Western diet: health implications for the 21st century".Am. J. Clin. Nutr.81 (2):341–54.doi:10.1093/ajcn.81.2.341.PMID 15699220.
  141. ^Ulijaszek SJ (November 2002)."Human eating behaviour in an evolutionary ecological context".Proc Nutr Soc.61 (4):517–26.doi:10.1079/PNS2002180.PMID 12691181.
  142. ^Earliest agriculture in the AmericasArchived 3 June 2010 at theWayback MachineEarliest cultivation of barleyArchived 16 February 2007 at theWayback MachineEarliest cultivation of figsArchived 2 June 2006 at theWayback Machine, retrieved 19 February 2007
  143. ^Krebs JR (September 2009)."The gourmet ape: evolution and human food preferences".Am. J. Clin. Nutr.90 (3):707S –11S.doi:10.3945/ajcn.2009.27462B.PMID 19656837.
  144. ^Holden C, Mace R (October 1997)."Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults".Hum. Biol.69 (5):605–28.PMID 9299882.
  145. ^Fichtel, Claudia (2012). "Predation". In Mitani, John C.; Call, Josep; Kappeler, Peter M.; Palombit, Ryne A.;Silk, Joan B. (eds.).The Evolution of Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press. pp. 169–84.ISBN 978-0-226-53172-4.
  146. ^Liman, E. R.; Innan, H. (2003)."Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution"(PDF).Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.100 (6):3328–3332.Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.3328L.doi:10.1073/pnas.0636123100.PMC 152292.PMID 12631698. Retrieved2008-07-23.
  147. ^Egnor, R.; Miller, C.; Hauser, M.D. (2004)."Nonhuman Primate Communication"(PDF).Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (2nd ed.). Elsevier.ISBN 0-08-044299-4. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2008-09-10.
  148. ^abPollick, A. S.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2007)."Ape gestures and language evolution".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.104 (19):8184–8189.Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.8184P.doi:10.1073/pnas.0702624104.PMC 1876592.PMID 17470779.
  149. ^Burrows, A. M. (2008)."The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance".BioEssays.30 (3):212–225.doi:10.1002/bies.20719.PMID 18293360.S2CID 205478149.
  150. ^Wright, E.; Grawunder, S; Ndayishimiye, E; Galbany, J; McFarlin, S. C.; Stoinski, T. S.; Robbins, M. M. (2021)."Chest beats as an honest signal of body size in male mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei)".Scientific Reports.11 (1): 6879.Bibcode:2021NatSR..11.6879W.doi:10.1038/s41598-021-86261-8.PMC 8032651.PMID 33833252.
  151. ^Geissmann, Thomas & Mutschler, Thomas (2006)."Diurnal Distribution of Loud Calls in Sympatric Wild Indris (Indri indri) and Ruffed Lemurs (Varecia variegata): Implications for Call Functions"(PDF).Primates; Journal of Primatology.47 (4):393–6.doi:10.1007/s10329-006-0189-5.PMID 16736264.S2CID 1586657.
  152. ^Ramsier, M.A.; Cunningham, A.J.; Moritz, G.L.; Finneran, J.J.; Williams, C.V.; Ong, P.S.; Gursky-Doyen, S.L.; Dominy, N.J. (2012)."Primate communication in the pure ultrasound".Biology Letters.8 (4):508–511.doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.1149.PMC 3391437.PMID 22319094.
  153. ^da Cunha, R. G. T.; Byrne, R. (2006). "Roars of Black Howler Monkeys (Alouatta caraya): Evidence for a Function in Inter-Group Spacing".Behaviour.143 (10):1169–1199.doi:10.1163/156853906778691568.JSTOR 4536401.
  154. ^"Black howler monkey". Smithsonian's National Zoo & Conservation Biology Institute. 25 April 2016. Retrieved2016-07-10.
  155. ^Haimoff, E. H. (1983). "Brief report: Occurrence of anti-resonance in the song of the siamang (Hylobates syndactylus)".American Journal of Primatology.5 (3):249–256.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350050309.PMID 31986856.S2CID 85262432.
  156. ^Seyfarth, R. M.; Cheney, D. L.; Marler, Peter (1980)."Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic communication in a Free-Ranging Primate".Animal Behaviour.28 (4):1070–1094.doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2.S2CID 53165940.
  157. ^Leroux, M.; Schel, A.M.; Wilke, C.; Chandia, B.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Slocombe, K.E.; Townsend, S. (2023)."Call combinations and compositional processing in wild chimpanzees".Nature Communications.14 (1): 2225.Bibcode:2023NatCo..14.2225L.doi:10.1038/s41467-023-37816-y.PMC 10160036.PMID 37142584.
  158. ^Coye, C.; Ouattara, K.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Lemasson, A. (2015)."Suffixation influences receivers' behaviour in non-human primates".Proceedings of the Royal Society B.282 (1807): 20150265.doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.0265.PMC 4424650.PMID 25925101.
  159. ^Coye, C.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Lemasson, A. (2016). "Morphologically structured vocalizations in female Diana monkeys".Animal Behaviour.115:97–105.doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.010.hdl:10023/10629.
  160. ^Leroux, M.; Townsend, S. (2020). "Call Combinations in Great Apes and the Evolution of Syntax".Animal Behavior and Cognition.7 (2):131–139.doi:10.26451/abc.07.02.07.2020.
  161. ^Lameira, A. R.; et al. (2021)."Orangutan information broadcast via consonant-like and vowel-like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution".Biology Letters.17 (9).doi:10.1098/rsbl.2021.0302.PMC 8478518.PMID 34582737.
  162. ^Arcadi, AC. (Aug 2000)."Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees: implications for the evolution of language".J Hum Evol.39 (2):205–23.Bibcode:2000JHumE..39..205A.doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0415.PMID 10968929.S2CID 7403772.
  163. ^Boesch, C.; Boesch, H. (1990). "Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees".Folia Primatologica.54 (1–2):86–99.doi:10.1159/000156428.PMID 2157651.
  164. ^Westergaard, G. C.; Lundquist, A. L.; et al. (1998)."Why some capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use probing tools (and others do not)".Journal of Comparative Psychology.112 (2):207–211.doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.207.PMID 9642788.
  165. ^de Waal, F. B. M.; Davis, J. M. (2003). "Capuchin cognitive ecology: cooperation based on projected returns".Neuropsychologia.41 (2):221–228.doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00152-5.PMID 12459220.S2CID 8190458.
  166. ^Paar, L. A.; Winslow, J. T.; Hopkins, W. D.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2000)."Recognizing facial cues: Individual discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)".Journal of Comparative Psychology.114 (1):47–60.doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.47.PMC 2018744.PMID 10739311.
  167. ^Byrne, Richard; Corp, Nadia (2004)."Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates".Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.271 (1549):1693–1699.doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2780.PMC 1691785.PMID 15306289.
  168. ^Paar, L. A.; de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). "Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees".Nature.399 (6737):647–648.Bibcode:1999Natur.399..647P.doi:10.1038/21345.PMID 10385114.S2CID 4424086.
  169. ^Fujita, K.; Watanabe, K.; Widarto, T. H.; Suryobroto, B. (1997). "Discrimination of macaques by macaques: The case of sulawesi species".Primates.38 (3):233–245.doi:10.1007/BF02381612.S2CID 21042762.
  170. ^Call, J. (2001)."Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens)".Journal of Comparative Psychology.115 (2):159–171.doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.159.PMID 11459163.
  171. ^Itakura, S.; Tanaka, M. (June 1998)."Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens)".Journal of Comparative Psychology.112 (2):119–126.doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.119.PMID 9642782.
  172. ^Gouteux, S.; Thinus-Blanc, C.; Vauclair, J. (2001)."Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task"(PDF).Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.130 (3):505–519.doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.505.PMID 11561924.
  173. ^Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (1997).Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press US.ISBN 978-0-19-510624-4.
  174. ^abcDeaner, R. O.; van Schaik, C. P.; Johnson, V. E. (2006)."Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A metaanalysis of nonhuman primate studies".Evolutionary Psychology.4:149–196.doi:10.1177/147470490600400114.S2CID 16702785.
  175. ^abcReader, S. M.; Hager, Y.; Laland, K. N. (2011)."The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence"(PDF).Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.366 (1567):1017–1027.doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0342.PMC 3049098.PMID 21357224. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2011-10-03. Retrieved2011-07-04.
  176. ^ab"Toolmaking". The Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved2013-08-01.
  177. ^"Bonobos". ApeTag. 2010. Archived fromthe original on 2013-11-02. Retrieved2013-08-03.
  178. ^Gruber, T.; Clay, Z.; Zuberbühler, K. (2010)."A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in thePan lineage"(PDF).Animal Behaviour.80 (6):1023–1033.doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.005.S2CID 14923158.
  179. ^Bower, B. (18 April 2011)."Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish".Wired. Retrieved2013-08-05.
  180. ^Breuer, T.; Ndoundou-Hockemba, M.; Fishlock, V. (2005)."First observation of tool use in wild gorillas".PLOS Biology.3 (11): e380.doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030380.PMC 1236726.PMID 16187795.
  181. ^Boinski, S. (1988). "Use of a club by a wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper)".American Journal of Primatology.14 (2):177–179.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350140208.PMID 31973450.S2CID 84653622.
  182. ^Fragaszy, D.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E.; Ottoni, E.B.; de Oliveira, M.G. (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools".American Journal of Primatology.64 (4):359–366.doi:10.1002/ajp.20085.PMID 15580579.S2CID 16222308.
  183. ^Gumert, M.D.; Kluck, M.; Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). "The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand".American Journal of Primatology.71 (7):594–608.doi:10.1002/ajp.20694.PMID 19405083.S2CID 22384150.
  184. ^Hamilton, W.J.; Buskirk, R.E.; Buskirk, W.H. (1975). "Defensive stoning by baboons".Nature.256 (5517):488–489.Bibcode:1975Natur.256..488H.doi:10.1038/256488a0.S2CID 4149862.
  185. ^Fichtel, C.; Kappeler, P. M. (2010)."Chapter 19: Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies: Establishing the lemur baseline". In Kappeler, P. M.; Silk, J. B. (eds.).Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals. Springer.ISBN 978-3-642-02724-6.
  186. ^Sugiyama, Y. (1995). "Drinking tools of wild chimpanzees at Bossou".American Journal of Primatology.37 (1):263–269.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350370308.PMID 31936951.S2CID 86473603.
  187. ^"Sumatran orangutans". OrangutanIslands.com. Archived fromthe original on 2013-11-26. Retrieved2013-08-02.
  188. ^van Schaik, C.; Fox, E.; Sitompul, A. (1996)."Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans".Naturwissenschaften.83 (4):186–188.Bibcode:1996NW.....83..186V.doi:10.1007/BF01143062.PMID 8643126.S2CID 27180148.
  189. ^Gill, Victoria (22 July 2011)."Mandrill monkey makes 'pedicuring' tool". BBC. Retrieved2013-08-11.
  190. ^Vancatova, M. (2008)."Gorillas and Tools – Part I". Retrieved2013-08-04.
  191. ^Piantadosi CA (2003).The biology of human survival : life and death in extreme environments. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 2–3.ISBN 978-0-19-974807-5.OCLC 70215878.Archived from the original on 30 July 2022. Retrieved30 July 2022.
  192. ^Cowlishaw, G.; Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). "Primates". In MacDonald, D. (ed.).The Princeton Encyclopedia of Mammals. Princeton and Oxford University Press. pp. 270–280.ISBN 978-0-691-14069-8.
  193. ^Reed, K.; Fleagle, J. (August 15, 1995)."Geographic and climatic control of primate diversity".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.92 (17):7874–7876.Bibcode:1995PNAS...92.7874R.doi:10.1073/pnas.92.17.7874.PMC 41248.PMID 7644506.
  194. ^Chapman, C. & Russo, S. (2007). "Primate Seed Dispersal". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.).Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 510.ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  195. ^Long, Y. C.; Kirkpatrick, R. C.; Zhong, T.; Xiao, L. (April 1994). "Report on the distribution, population, and ecology of the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus bieti)".Primates.35 (2):241–250.doi:10.1007/BF02382060.S2CID 23931368.
  196. ^Schaller, G. B. (1963).The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago: University Chicago Press.ISBN 978-0-226-73635-8.
  197. ^Stammbach, E. (1987). "Desert, Forest, and Montane Baboons: Multilevel-Societies". In Smuts, B; Cheney, D; Seyfarth, R; Wrangham, R; Struhsaker, T. (eds.).Primate Societies. The University of Chicago Press. pp. 112–120.ISBN 978-0226767161.
  198. ^Kemp, E. (2009). "Patterns of Water Use in Primates".Folia Primatologica.80 (4):275–294.doi:10.1159/000252586.PMID 19864919.S2CID 5108827.
  199. ^Wolfe, L. D. & Fuentes, A. (2007). "Ethnoprimatology". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.).Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 692.ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  200. ^Renquist, D. M. & Whitney, R. A. (1987)."Zoonoses Acquired from Pet Primates".Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice.17 (1):219–240.doi:10.1016/s0195-5616(87)50614-3.PMID 3551307. Retrieved2008-08-11.
  201. ^"The Universal Declaration of Human Rights".United Nations. 1948. Archived fromthe original on 2014-12-08. Retrieved2008-12-02.
  202. ^abCavalieri, P. & Singer, P."Declaration on Great Apes".Great Ape Project. Archived fromthe original on 2008-08-20. Retrieved2008-06-16.
  203. ^Glendinning, L. (26 June 2008)."Spanish parliament approves 'human rights' for apes".The Guardian. Retrieved2008-11-10.
  204. ^Singer, P. (18 July 2008)."Of great apes and men".The Guardian. Retrieved2008-11-10.
  205. ^Mott, M. (16 September 2003)."The Perils of Keeping Monkeys as Pets".National Geographic. Archived fromthe original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved2013-02-06.
  206. ^abWorkman, C. (June 2004)."Primate conservation in Vietnam: toward a holistic environmental narrative".American Anthropologist.106 (2):346–352.doi:10.1525/aa.2004.106.2.346.
  207. ^"IPPL News: The US Pet Monkey Trade".International Primate Protection League. 2003. Archived from the original on 2008-07-25. Retrieved2008-08-04.
  208. ^Bushnell, D. (1958)."The Beginnings of Research in Space Biology at the Air Force Missile Development Center, 1946–1952".History of Research in Space Biology and Biodynamics.NASA. Archived fromthe original on 2008-01-25. Retrieved2008-08-18.
  209. ^Blumenthal, D. (1987-06-17)."Monkeys as Helpers To Quadriplegics At Home".The New York Times. Retrieved2008-10-08.
  210. ^Newman, James L. (2013).Encountering Gorillas: A Chronicle of Discovery, Exploitation, Understanding, and Survival. Plymouth, United Kingdom: Rowman and Littlefield. p. 173.ISBN 978-1-4422-1957-1.
  211. ^ab"The supply and use of primates in the EU". European Biomedical Research Association. 1996. Archived fromthe original on 2012-01-17. Retrieved2008-08-18.
  212. ^Chen, F. C.; Li, W. H. (February 2001)."Genomic divergences between humans and other hominoids and the effective population size of the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees".American Journal of Human Genetics.68 (2):444–456.doi:10.1086/318206.PMC 1235277.PMID 11170892.
  213. ^Conlee, K. M.; Hoffeld, E. H.; Stephens, M. L. (2004)."A Demographic Analysis of Primate Research in the United States".Alternatives to Laboratory Animals.32 (Sup 1):315–322.doi:10.1177/026119290403201s52.PMID 23577480.S2CID 6743973.
  214. ^Presented to Parliament by the Secretary of State for the Home Department by Command of Her Majesty (July 2006).Statistics of scientific procedures on living animals: Great Britain 2005(PDF).The Stationery Office.ISBN 0-10-168772-9. Retrieved2008-06-16.
  215. ^"Nonhuman Primates: Research Animals".Animal Welfare Information Center. United States Department of Agriculture. Archived fromthe original on 2008-10-03. Retrieved2008-07-14.
  216. ^"Directive 86/609". European Coalition to End Animal Experiments. Archived fromthe original on 2008-09-29. Retrieved2008-10-08.
  217. ^abEstrada, Alejandro; Garber, Paul A.; Rylands, Anthony B.; Roos, Christian; Fernandez-Duque, Eduardo; Fiore, Anthony Di; Nekaris, K. Anne-Isola; Nijman, Vincent; Heymann, Eckhard W. (2017-01-01)."Impending extinction crisis of the world's primates: Why primates matter".Science Advances.3 (1): e1600946.Bibcode:2017SciA....3E0946E.doi:10.1126/sciadv.1600946.ISSN 2375-2548.PMC 5242557.PMID 28116351.
  218. ^IFAW (2005).Born to be wild: Primates are not pets(PDF). International Fund for Animal Welfare. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2011-07-26. Retrieved2011-02-26.
  219. ^CITES (2010-10-14)."Appendices I, II and III". Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora. Retrieved2012-04-02.
  220. ^Grubb, P. (1998). "The Sierra Leone monkey drives".Mammals of Ghana, Sierra Leone, and the Gambia. St. Ives: Trendrine. pp. 214–219.ISBN 0-9512562-4-6.
  221. ^abChapman, C. A.; Peres, C. A. (2001). "Primate conservation in the new millennium: the role of scientists".Evolutionary Anthropology.10 (1):16–33.doi:10.1002/1520-6505(2001)10:1<16::AID-EVAN1010>3.0.CO;2-O.S2CID 13494366.
  222. ^abMittermeier, R. A. & Cheney, D. L. (1987). "Conservation of primates and their habitats". In Smuts, B. B.; Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M.; Wrangham, R. W. & Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.).Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 477–490.
  223. ^abSouthwick, C. H. & Siddiqi, M. F. (2001)."Status, conservation and management of primates in India"(PDF).Envis Bulletin: Wildlife and Protected Areas.1 (1):81–91. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2008-10-01. Retrieved2008-08-04.
  224. ^abcdCowlishaw, G. & Dunbar, R. (2000).Primate Conservation Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.ISBN 978-0-226-11637-2.
  225. ^Van Schaik, C. P.; Monk, K. A.; Robertson, J. M. Y. (2001)."Dramatic decline in orangutan numbers in the Leuser Ecosystem, northern Sumatra".Oryx.35 (1):14–25.doi:10.1046/j.1365-3008.2001.00150.x.
  226. ^Purvis, A.; Gittleman, J. L.; Cowlishaw, G.; Mace, G. M. (2000)."Predicting extinction risk in declining species".Proceedings of the Royal Society B.267 (1456):1947–1952.doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1234.PMC 1690772.PMID 11075706.
  227. ^abcFa, J. E.; Juste, J.; Perez de Val, J.; Castroviejo, J. (1995)."Impact of market hunting on mammal species in Equatorial Guinea".Conservation Biology.9 (5):1107–1115.doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.1995.9051107.x.hdl:10261/49187.PMID 34261280.
  228. ^Hill, C. M. (1997). "Crop-raiding by wild vertebrates: The farmer's perspective in an agricultural community in western Uganda".International Journal of Pest Management.43 (1):77–84.doi:10.1080/096708797229022.
  229. ^Hill, C. M. (2002)."Primate conservation and local communities: Ethical issues and debates".American Anthropologist.104 (4):1184–1194.doi:10.1525/aa.2002.104.4.1184.
  230. ^Choudhury, A. (2001)."Primates in Northeast India: an overview of their distribution and conservation status"(PDF).Envis Bulletin: Wildlife and Protected Areas.1 (1):92–101. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2008-10-01. Retrieved2008-08-04.
  231. ^Kumara, H. N.; Singh, M. (October 2004). "Distribution and abundance of primates in rainforests of the Western Ghats, Karnataka, India and the conservation ofMacaca silenus".International Journal of Primatology.25 (5):1001–1018.doi:10.1023/B:IJOP.0000043348.06255.7f.S2CID 30384142.
  232. ^Nijman, V. (2004)."Conservation of the Javan gibbonHylobates moloch: population estimates, local extinction, and conservation priorities"(PDF).The Raffles Bulletin of Zoology.52 (1):271–280. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 2008-09-10. Retrieved2008-08-04.
  233. ^O'Brien, T. G.; Kinnaird, M. F.; Nurcahyo, A.; Iqbal, M.; Rusmanto, M. (April 2004). "Abundance and distribution of sympatric gibbons in a threatened Sumatran rain forest".International Journal of Primatology.25 (2):267–284.doi:10.1023/B:IJOP.0000019152.83883.1c.S2CID 32472118.
  234. ^Estrada, A.; Coates-Estrada, R.; Meritt, D. (September 1994). "Non-flying mammals and landscape changes in the tropical forest region of Los Tuxtlas, Mexico".Ecography.17 (3):229–241.doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.1994.tb00098.x.
  235. ^Marsh, L. K. (2003). "The nature of fragmentation.". In Marsh, L. K. (ed.).Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation. New York:Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 1–10.ISBN 0-306-47696-7.
  236. ^Turner, I. M. (1996)."Species loss in fragments of tropical rain forest: a review of the evidence".Journal of Applied Ecology.33 (2):200–209.Bibcode:1996JApEc..33..200T.doi:10.2307/2404743.JSTOR 2404743.
  237. ^Chiarello, A.G. (2003). "Primates of the Brazilian Atlantic forest: the influence of forest fragmentation on survival". In Marsh, L. K. (ed.).Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 99–121.ISBN 978-0-306-47696-9.
  238. ^Pope, T.R. (1996). "Socioecology, population fragmentation, and patterns of genetic loss in endangered primates". In Avise, J.; Hamrick, J. (eds.).Conservation Genetics: Case Histories from Nature. Norwell: Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 119–159.ISBN 978-0-412-05581-2.
  239. ^Mittermeier, R.A.; Wallis, J.; Rylands, A.B.; Ganzhorn, J.U.; Oates, J.F.; Williamson, E.A.; Palacios, E.; Heymann, E.W.; Kierulff, M.C.M.; Yongcheng, L.; Supriatna, J.; Roos, C.; Walker, S.; Cortés-Ortiz, L.; Schwitzer, C., eds. (2009).Primates in Peril: The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates 2008–2010(PDF). Illustrated by S.D. Nash. Arlington, VA.: IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), and Conservation International (CI). pp. 23–26.ISBN 978-1-934151-34-1.
  240. ^Oates, J. F.; Abedi-Lartey, M.; McGraw, W. S.; Struhsaker, T. T.; Whitesides, G. H. (October 2000). "Extinction of a West African Red Colobus Monkey".Conservation Biology.14 (5):1526–1532.Bibcode:2000ConBi..14.1526O.doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.2000.99230.x.S2CID 84336872.
  241. ^McGraw, W. S. (June 2005). "Update on the Search for Miss Waldron's Red Colobus Monkey".International Journal of Primatology.26 (3):605–619.doi:10.1007/s10764-005-4368-9.S2CID 861418.

Literature cited

Further reading

Library resources about
Primates
  • David J. Chivers; Bernard A. Wood; Alan Bilsborough, eds. (1984).Food Acquisition and Processing in Primates. New York & London: Plenum Press.ISBN 0-306-41701-4.

External links

Wikisource has the text of the1911Encyclopædia Britannica article "Primates".
Extantmammal orders
Yinotheria
Australosphenida
Theria
Metatheria
(Marsupial inclusive)
Ameridelphia
Australidelphia
Eutheria
(Placental inclusive)
Atlantogenata
Xenarthra
Afrotheria
Boreoeutheria
Laurasiatheria
Euarchontoglires
Extantprimate families
Strepsirrhini
Lorisoidea
Lemuroidea
Chiromyiformes
Haplorhini
Simian
Platyrrhini
Catharrhini
Hominoidea
Extant
ape species
Study of apes
Legal and
social status
Related
Primates
Authority control databases: NationalEdit this at Wikidata

Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Primate&oldid=1278917798"
Categories:
Hidden categories:

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp