Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Jump to content
WikipediaThe Free Encyclopedia
Search

Language family

Checked
Page protected with pending changes
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Page version status

This is an accepted version of this page

This is thelatest accepted revision,reviewed on15 July 2025.
Group of languages related through a common ancestor

2005 map of the contemporary distribution of the world's primary language families

Alanguage family is a group oflanguages related through descent from a common ancestor, called theproto-language of that family. The termfamily is a metaphor borrowed from biology, with thetree model used inhistorical linguistics analogous to afamily tree, or tophylogenetic trees of taxa used in evolutionarytaxonomy. Linguists thus describe thedaughter languages within a language family as beinggenetically related.[1] The divergence of a proto-language into daughter languages typically occurs through geographical separation, with differentregional dialects of the proto-language undergoing differentlanguage changes and thus becoming distinct languages over time.[2]

One well-known example of a language family is theRomance languages, includingSpanish,French,Italian,Portuguese,Romanian,Catalan,Romansh, and many others, all of which are descended fromVulgar Latin.[note 1][3] The Romance family itself is part of the largerIndo-European family, which includes many other languages native toEurope andSouth Asia, all believed to have descended from a common ancestor known asProto-Indo-European.

A language family is usually said to contain at least two languages, althoughlanguage isolates — languages that are not related to any other language — are occasionally referred to as families that contain one language. Conversely, there is no upper bound to the number of languages a family can contain. Some families, such as theAustronesian languages, contain over 1000.[4]

Language families can be identified from characteristics shared amongst their languages.Sound changes are one of the strongest pieces of evidence that can be used to identify a genetic relationship because of their predictable and consistent nature, and through thecomparative method can be used to reconstruct proto-languages. However, languages can also change throughlanguage contact, which can falsely suggest genetic relationships. For example, theMongolic,Tungusic, andTurkic languages share many similarities that have led several scholars to believethey were related. These supposed relationships were later discovered (in the view of most scholars) to be derived through language contact and thus they are not related through shared ancestry.[5] Eventually though, intense language contact with other language families, and inconsistent changes within the original language family, will obscure inherited characteristics and make it virtually impossible to deduce earlier relationships; even the oldest demonstrable language family,Afroasiatic, is far younger than language itself.[6]

Major language families

[edit]
Main article:List of language families

Estimates of the number of language families in the world may vary widely. According toEthnologue there are 7,151living human languages across 142 different language families.[7][8]Lyle Campbell (2019) identifies a total of 406 independent language families, including isolates.[9]

Ethnologue 27 (2024) lists the following families that contain at least 1% of the 7,164 known languages in the world;[10] whileGlottolog 5.0 (2024) lists the following as the largest families, of 7,788 languages (other thansign languages,pidgins, andunclassifiable languages):[11]

Ethnologue 27
FamilyNo. of
languages
Niger–Congo1,552
Austronesian1,256
Trans–New Guinea481
Sino-Tibetan458
Indo-European454
Australian384
Afroasiatic382
Nilo-Saharan210
Otomanguean179
Austroasiatic167
Kra-Dai91
Dravidian85
Glottolog 5.0
FamilyNo. of
languages
Atlantic–Congo1,410
Austronesian1,274
Indo-European586
Sino-Tibetan514
Afroasiatic381
Trans–New Guinea316
Pama–Nyungan250
Otomanguean181
Austroasiatic158
Tai–Kadai95
Dravidian85
Arawakan77

Language counts can vary significantly depending on what is considered merely a dialect; for exampleLyle Campbell counts only 27 Otomanguean languages, although he,Ethnologue andGlottolog also disagree as to which languages belong in the family.

Neither Ethnologue[12] nor Glottolog[13] list the language families ofsign languages. Of sign language families, theFrancosign family, hosting around 70 languages, is by far the largest and most widespread.

The largest five language families in terms of number of speakers (Indo-European, Sino-Tibetan, Afro-Asiatic, Niger-Congo and Austronesian) make up five-sixths (almost 83.3%) of the world’s population.[8]

Genetic relationship

[edit]

Two languages have agenetic relationship, and belong to the same language family, if both are descended from a common ancestor through the process oflanguage change, or one is descended from the other.The term and the process of language evolution are independent of, and not reliant on, the terminology, understanding, and theories related togenetics in the biological sense, so, to avoid confusion, some linguists prefer the termgenealogical relationship.[14][15]: 184 

There is a remarkably similar pattern shown by the linguistic tree and the genetic tree of human ancestry[16] that was verified statistically.[17] Languages interpreted in terms of the putative phylogenetic tree of human languages are transmitted to a great extent vertically (by ancestry) as opposed to horizontally (by spatial diffusion).[18]

Establishment

[edit]
Main article:Comparative method

In some cases, the shared derivation of a group of related languages from a common ancestor is directlyattested in the historical record. For example, this is the case for theRomance language family, whereinSpanish,Italian,Portuguese,Romanian, andFrench are all descended from Latin, as well as for theNorth Germanic language family, includingDanish,Swedish,Norwegian andIcelandic, which have shared descent fromAncient Norse. Latin and ancient Norse are both attested in written records, as are many intermediate stages between those ancestral languages and their modern descendants.

In other cases, genetic relationships between languages are not directly attested. For instance, the Romance languages and the North Germanic languages are also related to each other, being subfamilies of theIndo-European language family, since both Latin and Old Norse are believed to be descended from an even more ancient language,Proto-Indo-European; however, no direct evidence of Proto-Indo-European or its divergence into its descendant languages survives. In cases such as these, genetic relationships are established through use of thecomparative method of linguistic analysis.

In order to test the hypothesis that two languages are related, the comparative method begins with the collection of pairs of words that are hypothesized to becognates: i.e., words in related languages that are derived from the same word in the shared ancestral language. Pairs of words that have similar pronunciations and meanings in the two languages are often good candidates for hypothetical cognates. The researcher must rule out the possibility that the two words are similar merely due to chance, or due to one havingborrowed the words from the other (or from a language related to the other). Chance resemblance is ruled out by the existence of large collections of pairs of words between the two languages showing similar patterns of phonetic similarity. Once coincidental similarity andborrowing have been eliminated as possible explanations for similarities in sound and meaning of words, the remaining explanation is common origin: it is inferred that the similarities occurred due to descent from a common ancestor, and the words are actually cognates, implying the languages must be related.[19]

Linguistic interference and borrowing

[edit]

When languages are incontact with one another, either of them may influence the other throughlinguistic interference such as borrowing. For example,French has influencedEnglish,Arabic has influencedPersian,Sanskrit has influencedTamil, andChinese has influencedJapanese in this way. However, such influence does not constitute (and is not a measure of) a genetic relationship between the languages concerned. Linguistic interference can occur between languages that are genetically closely related, between languages that are distantly related (like English and French, which are distantly relatedIndo-European languages) and between languages that have no genetic relationship.

Complications

[edit]

Some exceptions to the simple genetic relationship model of languages includelanguage isolates andmixed,pidgin andcreole languages.

Mixed languages, pidgins and creole languages constitute special genetic types of languages. They do not descend linearly or directly from a single language and have no single ancestor.

Isolates are languages that cannot be proven to be genealogically related to any other modern language. As a corollary, every language isolate also forms its own language family — a genetic family which happens to consist of just one language. One often cited example isBasque, which forms a language family on its own; but there are many other examples outside Europe. On the global scale, the siteGlottolog counts a total of 423 language families in the world, including 184 isolates.[20]

Monogenesis

[edit]

One controversial theory concerning the genetic relationships among languages ismonogenesis, the idea that all known languages, with the exceptions ofcreoles, pidgins andsign languages, are descendant from a single ancestral language.[21] If that is true, it would mean all languages (other than pidgins, creoles, and sign languages) are genetically related, but in many cases, the relationships may be too remote to be detectable. Alternative explanations for some basic observed commonalities between languages include developmental theories, related to the biological development of the capacity for language as the child grows from newborn.[citation needed]

Structure of a family

[edit]
This sectionneeds additional citations forverification. Please helpimprove this article byadding citations to reliable sources in this section. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.(May 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

A language family is amonophyletic unit; all its members derive from a common ancestor, and all descendants of that ancestor are included in the family. Thus, the termfamily is analogous to the biological termclade. Language families can be divided into smaller phylogenetic units, sometimes referred to as "branches" or "subfamilies" of the family; for instance, theGermanic languages are a subfamily of theIndo-European family. Subfamilies share a more recent common ancestor than the common ancestor of the larger family;Proto-Germanic, the common ancestor of the Germanic subfamily, was itself a descendant ofProto-Indo-European, the common ancestor of the Indo-European family. Within a large family, subfamilies can be identified through "shared innovations": members of a subfamily will share features that represent retentions from their more recent common ancestor, but were not present in the overall proto-language of the larger family.

Sometaxonomists restrict the termfamily to a certain level, but there is little consensus on how to do so. Those who affix such labels also subdivide branches intogroups, and groups intocomplexes. A top-level (i.e., the largest) family is often called aphylum orstock. The closer the branches are to each other, the more closely the languages will be related. This means if a branch of aproto-language is four branches down and there is also asister language to that fourth branch, then the two sister languages are more closely related to each other than to that common ancestral proto-language.

The termmacrofamily orsuperfamily is sometimes applied to proposed groupings of language families whose status as phylogenetic units is generally considered to be unsubstantiated by acceptedhistorical linguistic methods.

Dialect continua

[edit]
Main article:Dialect continuum

Some close-knit language families, and many branches within larger families, take the form ofdialect continua in which there are no clear-cut geographical boundaries that make it possible to unequivocally identify, define, or count individual languages within the family. However, when the differences between the speech of different regions at the extremes of the continuum are so great that there is nomutual intelligibility between them, as occurs inArabic, the continuum cannot meaningfully be seen as a single language.

Further, a speech variety may either be considered as a language or as a dialect, depending on social or political considerations. Thus, different sources, especially over time, can give wildly different numbers of languages within a certain family.Classifications of the Japonic family, for example, range from one language (a language isolate with dialects) to nearly twenty—until the classification ofRyukyuan as separate languages within aJaponic language family rather than dialects of Japanese, theJapanese language itself was considered alanguage isolate and therefore the only language in its family.

Isolates

[edit]
Main article:Language isolate

Most of the world's languages are known to be related to others. Those that have no known relatives (or for which family relationships are only tentatively proposed) are calledlanguage isolates, essentially language families consisting of a single language. There are an estimated 129 language isolates known today.[22] An example isBasque. In general, it is assumed that language isolates have relatives or had relatives at some point in their history but at a time depth too great for linguistic comparison to recover them.

A language isolate is classified based on the fact that enough is known about the isolate to compare it genetically to other languages but no common ancestry or relationship is found with any other known language.[22]

A language isolated in its own branch within a family, such asAlbanian andArmenian within Indo-European, is often also called an isolate, but the meaning of the word "isolate" in such cases is usually clarified with amodifier. For instance, Albanian and Armenian may be referred to as an "Indo-European isolate". By contrast, so far as is known, theBasque language is an absolute isolate: it has not been shown to be related to any other modern language despite numerous attempts. A language may be said to be an isolate currently but not historically if related but now extinct relatives are attested. TheAquitanian language, spoken in Roman times, may have been an ancestor of Basque, but it could also have been a sister language to the ancestor of Basque. In the latter case, Basque and Aquitanian would form a small family together. Ancestors are not considered to be distinct members of a family.[citation needed]

Proto-languages

[edit]
Main article:Proto-language

A proto-language can be thought of as a mother language (not to be confused with amother tongue[23]) being the root from which all languages in the family stem. The common ancestor of a language family is seldom known directly since most languages have a relatively short recorded history. However, it is possible to recover many features of a proto-language by applying thecomparative method, a reconstructive procedure worked out by 19th century linguistAugust Schleicher. This can demonstrate the validity of many of the proposed families in thelist of language families. For example, the reconstructible common ancestor of the Indo-European language family is calledProto-Indo-European. Proto-Indo-European is not attested by written records and so is conjectured to have been spoken before the invention of writing.

Visual representation

[edit]
An example of a language tree, containing the Mayan languages

A common visual representation of a language family is given by a genetic language tree. Thetree model is sometimes termed adendrogram orphylogeny. The family tree shows the relationship of the languages within a family, much as a family tree of an individual shows their relationship with their relatives. There are criticisms to the family tree model. Critics focus mainly on the claim that the internal structure of the trees is subject to variation based on the criteria of classification.[24] Even among those who support the family tree model, there are debates over which languages should be included in a language family. For example, within the dubiousAltaic language family, there are debates over whether theJaponic andKoreanic languages should be included or not.[25]

Thewave model has been proposed as an alternative to the tree model.[15] The wave model usesisoglosses to group language varieties; unlike in the tree model, these groups can overlap. While the tree model implies a lack of contact between languages after derivation from an ancestral form, the wave model emphasizes the relationship between languages that remain in contact, which is more realistic.[15]Historical glottometry is an application of the wave model, meant to identify and evaluate genetic relations inlinguistic linkages.[15][26]

Other classifications of languages

[edit]
This sectionneeds additional citations forverification. Please helpimprove this article byadding citations to reliable sources in this section. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed.(May 2022) (Learn how and when to remove this message)

Sprachbund

[edit]
Main article:Sprachbund

Asprachbund is a geographic area having several languages that feature common linguistic structures. The similarities between those languages are caused by language contact, not by chance or common origin, and are not recognized as criteria that define a language family. An example of a sprachbund would be theIndian subcontinent.[27]

Shared innovations, acquired by borrowing or other means, are not considered genetic and have no bearing with the language family concept. It has been asserted, for example, that many of the more striking features shared byItalic languages (Latin,Oscan,Umbrian, etc.) might well be "areal features". However, very similar-looking alterations in the systems of long vowels in theWest Germanic languages greatly postdate any possible notion of a proto-language innovation (and cannot readily be regarded as "areal", either, since English and continental West Germanic were not a linguistic area). In a similar vein, there are many similar unique innovations inGermanic,Baltic andSlavic that are far more likely to be areal features than traceable to a common proto-language. But legitimate uncertainty about whether shared innovations are areal features, coincidence, or inheritance from a common ancestor, leads to disagreement over the proper subdivisions of any large language family.

Contact languages

[edit]
Main articles:Mixed language,Creole language, andPidgin

The concept of language families is based on the historical observation that languages developdialects, which over time may diverge into distinct languages. However, linguistic ancestry is less clear-cut than familiar biological ancestry, in which species do not crossbreed.[28] It is more like the evolution of microbes, with extensivelateral gene transfer. Quite distantly related languages may affect each other throughlanguage contact, which in extreme cases may lead to languages with no single ancestor, whether they becreoles ormixed languages. In addition, a number ofsign languages have developed in isolation and appear to have no relatives at all. Nonetheless, such cases are relatively rare and most well-attested languages can be unambiguously classified as belonging to one language family or another, even if this family's relation to other families is not known.

Language contact can lead to the development of new languages from the mixture of two or more languages for the purposes of interactions between two groups who speak different languages. Languages that arise in order for two groups to communicate with each other to engage in commercial trade or that appeared as a result of colonialism are calledpidgin. Pidgins are an example of linguistic and cultural expansion caused by language contact. However, language contact can also lead to cultural divisions. In some cases, two different language speaking groups can feel territorial towards their language and do not want any changes to be made to it. This causes language boundaries and groups in contact are not willing to make any compromises to accommodate the other language.[29]

See also

[edit]

Notes

[edit]
  1. ^VernacularLatin, as opposed to theClassical Latin used as a literary language.

References

[edit]
  1. ^Rowe, Bruce M.; Levine, Diane P. (2015).A Concise Introduction to Linguistics. Routledge. pp. 340–341.ISBN 978-1-317-34928-0. Retrieved26 January 2017.
  2. ^Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (2011).Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages. John Benjamins. p. 336.ISBN 978-9-027-28722-9. Retrieved26 January 2017.
  3. ^Lewis, M. Paul, Gary F. Simons, and Charles D. Fennig (eds.).Ethnologue: Languages of the World, Seventeenth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International, 2013.
  4. ^"Family: Austronesian".Glottolog 5.0. Retrieved3 August 2024.
  5. ^De la Fuente, José Andrés Alonso (2016)."Review of Robbeets, Martine (2015): Diachrony of verb morphology. Japanese and the Transeurasian languages".Diachronica.33 (4):530–537.doi:10.1075/dia.33.4.04alo.
  6. ^Boë, Louis-Jean; et al. (11 December 2019)."Which way to the dawn of speech?: Reanalyzing half a century of debates and data in light of speech science".Science.5 (12).doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaw3916.PMC 7000245.
  7. ^"How many languages are there in the world?".Ethnologue. 3 May 2016. Retrieved26 March 2021.
  8. ^ab"What are the largest language families?".Ethnologue. 25 May 2019. Retrieved3 March 2020.
  9. ^Campbell, Lyle (8 January 2019)."How many language families are there in the world?".Anuario del Seminario de Filología Vasca "Julio de Urquijo".52 (1/2). UPV/EHU Press: 133.doi:10.1387/asju.20195.hdl:10810/49565.ISSN 2444-2992.S2CID 166394477.
  10. ^"Welcome to the 24th edition".Ethnologue. 13 April 2024.
  11. ^"Glottolog 5.0 -".glottolog.org. Retrieved25 June 2023.
  12. ^"Sign Language".ethnologue.com. Retrieved15 March 2025.
  13. ^"Pseudo Family: Sign Language".glottolog.org. Retrieved15 March 2025.
  14. ^Haspelmath, Martin (5 May 2004)."How hopeless is genealogical linguistics, and how advanced is areal linguistics? — Review of Aikhenvald & Dixon (2001): Areal diffusion and genetic inheritance".Studies in Language.28 (1):209–223.doi:10.1075/sl.28.1.10has. p. 222.
  15. ^abcdFrançois, Alexandre (2014)."Trees, Waves and Linkages: Models of Language Diversification"(PDF). In Bowern, Claire; Evans, Bethwyn (eds.).The Routledge Handbook of Historical Linguistics. London: Routledge. pp. 161–189.ISBN 978-0-41552-789-7.
  16. ^Henn, B. M.;Cavalli-Sforza, L. L.;Feldman, M. W. (17 October 2012)."The great human expansion".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.109 (44):17758–17764.Bibcode:2012PNAS..10917758H.doi:10.1073/pnas.1212380109.JSTOR 41829755.PMC 3497766.PMID 23077256.
  17. ^Cavalli-Sforza, L. L.; Minch, E.; Mountain, J. L. (15 June 1992)."Coevolution of genes and languages revisited".Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.89 (12):5620–5624.Bibcode:1992PNAS...89.5620C.doi:10.1073/pnas.89.12.5620.JSTOR 2359705.PMC 49344.PMID 1608971.
  18. ^Gell-Mann, M.;Ruhlen, M. (10 October 2011)."The origin and evolution of word order"(PDF).Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.108 (42):17290–17295.Bibcode:2011PNAS..10817290G.doi:10.1073/pnas.1113716108.JSTOR 41352497.PMC 3198322.PMID 21987807.
  19. ^Campbell, Lyle (2013).Historical Linguistics. MIT Press.
  20. ^Cf.Language families,Glottolog.
  21. ^Nichols, Johanna. Monogenesis or Polygenesis: A Single Ancestral Language for All Humanity? Ch. 58 ofThe Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution, ed. byMaggie Tallerman and Kathleen Rita Gibson. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2012. 558–72. Print.
  22. ^abCampbell, Lyle (24 August 2010)."Language Isolates and Their History, or, What's Weird, Anyway?".Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society.36 (1):16–31.doi:10.3765/bls.v36i1.3900.ISSN 2377-1666.
  23. ^Bloomfield, Leonard (1994).Language. Motilal Banarsidass Publ.ISBN 81-208-1196-8.
  24. ^Edzard, Lutz.Polygenesis, Convergence, and Entropy: An Alternative Model of Linguistic Evolution Applied to Semitic Linguistics. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz, 1998. Print.
  25. ^Georg, Stefan, Peter A. Michalove, Alexis Manaster Ramer, and Paul J. Sidwell. Telling General Linguists about Altaic.Journal of Linguistics 35.1 (1999): 65–98. Print.
  26. ^Kalyan, Siva; François, Alexandre (2018)."Freeing the Comparative Method from the tree model: A framework for Historical Glottometry"(PDF). In Kikusawa, Ritsuko; Reid, Laurie (eds.).Let's Talk about Trees: Genetic Relationships of Languages and Their Phylogenic Representation. Senri Ethnological Studies. Vol. 98. Ōsaka: National Museum of Ethnology. pp. 59–89.
  27. ^Joseph, Brian (2017)."The Balkan Sprachbund"(PDF).linguisticsociety.org. Retrieved2 October 2020.
  28. ^List, Johann-Mattis; Nelson-Sathi, Shijulal; Geisler, Hans; Martin, William (2014)."Networks of lexical borrowing and lateral gene transfer in language and genome evolution".BioEssays.36 (2):141–150.doi:10.1002/bies.201300096.ISSN 0265-9247.PMC 3910147.PMID 24375688.
  29. ^"Languages in Contact | Linguistic Society of America".www.linguisticsociety.org. Retrieved2 October 2020.

Further reading

[edit]
  • Boas, Franz (1911).Handbook of American Indian languages. Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Vol. 1. Washington:Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology.ISBN 0-8032-5017-7.{{cite book}}:ISBN / Date incompatibility (help)
  • Boas, Franz. (1922).Handbook of American Indian languages (Vol. 2). Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Washington, D.C.: Government Print Office (Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology).
  • Boas, Franz. (1933).Handbook of American Indian languages (Vol. 3). Native American legal materials collection, title 1227. Glückstadt: J.J. Augustin.
  • Campbell, Lyle. (1997).American Indian languages: The historical linguistics of Native America. New York: Oxford University Press.ISBN 0-19-509427-1.
  • Campbell, Lyle; & Mithun, Marianne (Eds.). (1979).The languages of native America: Historical and comparative assessment. Austin: University of Texas Press.
  • Goddard, Ives (Ed.). (1996).Languages. Handbook of North American Indians (W. C. Sturtevant, General Ed.) (Vol. 17). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution.ISBN 0-16-048774-9.
  • Goddard, Ives. (1999).Native languages and language families of North America (rev. and enlarged ed. with additions and corrections). [Map]. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press (Smithsonian Institution). (Updated version of the map in Goddard 1996).ISBN 0-8032-9271-6.
  • Gordon, Raymond G., Jr. (Ed.). (2005).Ethnologue: Languages of the world (15th ed.). Dallas, TX: SIL International.ISBN 1-55671-159-X. (Online version:Ethnologue: Languages of the World).
  • Greenberg, Joseph H. (1966).The Languages of Africa (2nd ed.). Bloomington: Indiana University.
  • Harrison, K. David. (2007)When Languages Die: The Extinction of the World's Languages and the Erosion of Human Knowledge. New York and London: Oxford University Press.
  • Mithun, Marianne. (1999).The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.ISBN 0-521-23228-7 (hbk);ISBN 0-521-29875-X.
  • Ross, Malcolm. (2005). "Pronouns as a preliminary diagnostic for grouping Papuan languagesArchived 8 June 2004 at theWayback Machine". In:Andrew Pawley, Robert Attenborough, Robin Hide and Jack Golson, eds,Papuan pasts: cultural, linguistic and biological histories of Papuan-speaking peoples (PDF)
  • Ruhlen, Merritt. (1987).A guide to the world's languages. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  • Sturtevant, William C. (Ed.). (1978–present).Handbook of North American Indians (Vol. 1–20). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. (Vols. 1–3, 16, 18–20 not yet published).
  • Voegelin, C. F. & Voegelin, F. M. (1977).Classification and index of the world's languages. New York: Elsevier.

External links

[edit]
Africa
Isolates
Eurasia
(Europe
andAsia)
Isolates
New Guinea
andthe Pacific
Isolates
Australia
Isolates
North
America
Isolates
Mesoamerica
Isolates
South
America
Isolates
Sign
languages
Isolates
See also
  • Families with question marks (?) are disputed or controversial.
  • Families initalics have no living members.
  • Families with more than 30 languages are inbold.
Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Language_family&oldid=1300590230"
Categories:
Hidden categories:

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp