Frequency-dependent selection is an evolutionary process by which thefitness of aphenotype orgenotype depends on the phenotype or genotype composition of a givenpopulation.
Frequency-dependent selection is usually the result of interactions between species (predation, parasitism, or competition), or between genotypes within species (usually competitive or symbiotic), and has been especially frequently discussed with relation toanti-predator adaptations. Frequency-dependent selection can lead topolymorphic equilibria, which result from interactions among genotypes within species, in the same way that multi-species equilibria require interactions between species in competition (e.g. whereαij parameters inLotka-Volterra competition equations are non-zero). Frequency-dependent selection can also lead todynamical chaos when some individuals' fitnesses become very low at intermediate allele frequencies.[2][3]
The first explicit statement of frequency-dependent selection appears to have been byEdward Bagnall Poulton in 1884, on the way that predators could maintain color polymorphisms in their prey.[5][6]
Perhaps the best known early modern statement of the principle isBryan Clarke's 1962 paper onapostatic selection (a form of negative frequency-dependent selection).[7] Clarke discussed predator attacks on polymorphic British snails, citingLuuk Tinbergen's classic work onsearching images as support that predators such as birds tended to specialize in common forms of palatable species.[8] Clarke later argued that frequency-dependent balancing selection could explain molecular polymorphisms (often in the absence ofheterosis) in opposition to theneutral theory of molecular evolution.[9]
Another example isplant self-incompatibilityalleles. When two plants share the same incompatibility allele, they are unable to mate. Thus, a plant with a new (and therefore, rare) allele has more success at mating, and its allele spreads quickly through the population.[10]
A similar example is the csd alleles of thehoney bee. A larva that is homozygous at csd is inviable. Therefore rare alleles spread through the population, pushing the gene pool toward an ideal equilibrium where every allele is equally common.[11]
Themajor histocompatibility complex (MHC) is involved in the recognition of foreign antigens and cells.[12] Frequency-dependent selection may explain the high degree of polymorphism in the MHC.[13]
Inbehavioral ecology, negative frequency-dependent selection often maintains multiple behavioral strategies within a species. A classic example is the Hawk-Dove model of interactions among individuals in a population. In a population with two traits A and B, being one form is better when most members are the other form. As another example, malecommon side-blotched lizards have three morphs, which either defend large territories and maintain large harems of females, defend smaller territories and keep one female, or mimic females in order to sneak matings from the other two morphs. These three morphs participate in arock paper scissors sort of interaction such that no one morph completely outcompetes the other two.[14][15] Another example occurs in thescaly-breasted munia, where certain individuals become scroungers and others become producers.[16]
Positive frequency-dependent selection gives an advantage to common phenotypes. A good example is warning coloration inaposematic species. Predators are more likely to remember a common color pattern that they have already encountered frequently than one that is rare. This means that new mutants or migrants that have color patterns other than the common type are eliminated from the population by differential predation. Positive frequency-dependent selection provides the basis forMüllerian mimicry, as described by Fritz Müller,[17] because all species involved are aposematic and share the benefit of a common, honest signal to potential predators.[citation needed]
Another, rather complicated example occurs in theBatesian mimicry complex between a harmless mimic, the scarlet kingsnake (Lampropeltis elapsoides), and the model, the eastern coral snake (Micrurus fulvius), in locations where the model and mimic were in deepsympatry, thephenotype of the scarlet kingsnake was quite variable due to relaxed selection. But where the pattern was rare, the predator population was not 'educated', so the pattern brought no benefit. The scarlet kingsnake was much less variable on the allopatry/sympatry border of the model and mimic, most probably due to increased selection since the eastern coral snake is rare, but present, on this border. Therefore, the coloration is only advantageous once it has become common.[18]