Movatterモバイル変換


[0]ホーム

URL:


Jump to content
WikipediaThe Free Encyclopedia
Search

Animal sexual behaviour

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Sexual behavior of non-human animals
This article is about the sexual behavior of nonhuman animals. For human sexual behavior, seeHuman sexual activity andHuman sexuality. For other uses, seeAnimal sex (disambiguation).

Bonobos mating in a zoo
Sexual orientation
Stags fighting while competing for females—a common sexual behavior
Anatomical structures on the head and throat of a domesticturkey. 1. Caruncles 2. Snood 3. Wattle (dewlap) 4. Major caruncle 5. Beard. During sexual behavior, these structures enlarge or become brightly colored.

Animal sexual behaviour takes many different forms, including within the samespecies. Commonmating orreproductively motivated systems includemonogamy,polygyny,polyandry,polygamy andpromiscuity. Other sexual behaviour may be reproductively motivated (e.g.sex apparently due to duress or coercion andsituational sexual behaviour) ornon-reproductively motivated (e.g.homosexual sexual behaviour,bisexual sexual behaviour,cross-species sex,sexual arousal from objects or places,sex with dead animals, etc.).

When animal sexual behaviour is reproductively motivated, it is often termedmating orcopulation; for most non-humanmammals, mating and copulation occur atoestrus (the most fertile period in the mammalian female's reproductive cycle), which increases the chances ofsuccessful impregnation.[1][2] Some animal sexual behaviour involvescompetition, sometimes fighting, between multiple males. Females often select males for mating only if they appear strong and able to protect themselves. The male that wins a fight may also have the chance to mate with a larger number of females and will therefore pass on his genes to their offspring.[3]

Historically, it was believed that only humans and a small number of other species performed sexual acts other than for reproduction, and that animals' sexuality was instinctive and a simple "stimulus-response" behaviour. However, in addition to homosexual behaviours, a range of speciesmasturbate and mayuse objects as tools to help them do so. Sexual behaviour may be tied more strongly to the establishment and maintenance of complex social bonds across a population which support its success in non-reproductive ways. Both reproductive and non-reproductive behaviours can be related to expressions of dominance over another animal or survival within a stressful situation (such as sex due to duress or coercion).

Mating systems

[edit]
Greater sage-grouse at alek, with multiple males displaying for the less conspicuous females

Insociobiology andbehavioural ecology, the term"mating system" is used to describe the ways in which animal societies are structured in relation to sexual behaviour. The mating system specifies which males mate with which females, and under what circumstances. There are four basic systems:

The four basic mating systems[4]: 160–161 [5]
Single femaleMultiple females
Single maleMonogamyPolygyny
Multiple malesPolyandryPolygynandry

Monogamy

[edit]
Main article:Monogamous pairing in animals
See also:Evolution of monogamy

Monogamy occurs when one male and one female mate exclusively with each other. A monogamous mating system is one in which individuals formlong-lasting pairs and cooperate in raising offspring. These pairs may last for a lifetime, such as inpigeons,[6] or it may occasionally change from one mating season to another, such as inemperor penguins.[7] In contrast withtournament species, these pair-bonding species have lower levels of male aggression, competition and littlesexual dimorphism.Zoologists andbiologists now have evidence that monogamous pairs of animals are not always sexually exclusive. Many animals that form pairs to mate and raise offspring regularly engage in sexual activities withextra-pair partners.[8][9][10][11] This includes previous examples, such asswans. Sometimes, theseextra-pair sexual activities lead to offspring. Genetic tests frequently show that some of the offspring raised by a monogamous pair come from the female mating with an extra-pair male partner.[9][12][13][14] These discoveries have led biologists to adopt new ways of talking about monogamy. According to Ulrich Reichard (2003):

Social monogamy refers to a male and female's social living arrangement (e.g., shared use of a territory, behaviour indicative of a social pair, and/or proximity between a male and female) without inferring any sexual interactions or reproductive patterns. In humans, social monogamy takes the form of monogamous marriage. Sexual monogamy is defined as an exclusive sexual relationship between a female and a male based on observations of sexual interactions. Finally, the term genetic monogamy is used when DNA analyses can confirm that a female-male pair reproduce exclusively with each other. A combination of terms indicates examples where levels of relationships coincide, e.g., sociosexual and sociogenetic monogamy describe corresponding social and sexual, and social and genetic monogamous relationships, respectively.[15]

Whatever makes a pair of animals socially monogamous does not necessarily make them sexually or genetically monogamous. Social monogamy, sexual monogamy, and genetic monogamy can occur in different combinations.

Social monogamy is relatively rare in the animal kingdom. The actual incidence of social monogamy varies greatly across different branches of the evolutionary tree. Over 90% of avian species are socially monogamous.[10][16] This stands in contrast to mammals. Only 3% of mammalian species are socially monogamous, although up to 15% of primate species are.[10][16] Social monogamy has also been observed inreptiles, fish, andinsects.

Sexual monogamy is also rare among animals. Many socially monogamous species engage inextra-pair copulations, making them sexually non-monogamous. For example, while over 90% of birds are socially monogamous, "on average, 30% or more of the baby birds in any nest [are] sired by someone other than the resident male."[17]Patricia Adair Gowaty has estimated that, out of 180 different species of socially monogamous songbirds, only 10% are sexually monogamous.[18]

The incidence of genetic monogamy, determined by DNA fingerprinting, varies widely across species. For a few rare species, the incidence of genetic monogamy is 100%, with all offspring genetically related to the socially monogamous pair. But genetic monogamy is strikingly low in other species. Barash and Lipton note:

The highest known frequency of extra-pair copulations are found among thefairy-wrens, lovely tropical creatures technically known asMalurus splendens andMalurus cyaneus. More than 65% of all fairy-wren chicks are fathered by males outside the supposed breeding group.[16]p. 12

Such low levels of genetic monogamy have surprised biologists and zoologists, forcing them to rethink the role of social monogamy in evolution. They can no longer assume social monogamy determines how genes are distributed in a species. The lower the rates of genetic monogamy among socially monogamous pairs, the less of a role social monogamy plays in determining how genes are distributed among offspring.

Polygamy

[edit]
See also:Promiscuity § Other animals

The termpolygamy is an umbrella term used to refer generally to non-monogamous matings. As such, polygamous relationships can be polygynous, polyandrous or polygynandrous. In a small number of species, individuals can display either polygamous or monogamous behaviour depending on environmental conditions. An example is the social waspApoica flavissima.[citation needed] In some species, polygyny and polyandry is displayed by both sexes in the population. Polygamy in both sexes has been observed inred flour beetle (Tribolium castaneum). Polygamy is also seen in manyLepidoptera species includingMythimna unipuncta (true armyworm moth).[19]

Atournament species is one in which "mating tends to be highly polygamous and involves high levels of male-male aggression and competition."[20] Tournament behaviour often correlates with high levels ofsexual dimorphism, examples of species includingchimpanzees andbaboons. Most polygamous species present high levels of tournament behaviour, with a notable exception beingbonobos.[citation needed]

Polygyny

[edit]
Main article:Polygyny in animals

Polygyny occurs when one male gets exclusive mating rights with multiple females. In some species, notably those withharem-like structures, only one of a few males in a group of females will mate. Technically,polygyny in sociobiology and zoology is defined as a system in which a male has a relationship with more than one female, but the females are predominantly bonded to a single male. Should the active male be driven out, killed, or otherwise removed from the group, in a number of species the new male will ensure that breeding resources are not wasted on another male's young.[21] The new male may achieve this in many different ways, including:

  • competitiveinfanticide: inlions,hippopotamuses, and somemonkeys, the new male will kill the offspring of the previous alpha male to cause their mothers to become receptive to his sexual advances since they are no longernursing. To prevent this, many female primates exhibit ovulation cues among all males, and show situation-dependent receptivity.[22]
  • harassment tomiscarriage: amongst wildhorses andbaboons, the male will continually attack pregnant females until they miscarry.
  • Pheromone-basedspontaneous abortion
  • in somerodents such asmice, a new male with a different scent will cause females who are pregnant to spontaneously fail to implant recently fertilised eggs. This does not require contact; it is mediated byscent alone. It is known as theBruce effect.

Von Haartman specifically described the mating behaviour of theEuropean pied flycatcher as successive polygyny.[23] Within this system, the males leave their home territory once their primary female lays her first egg. Males then create a second territory, presumably in order to attract a secondary female to breed. Even when they succeed at acquiring a second mate, the males typically return to the first female to exclusively provide for her and her offspring.[24]

Polygynous mating structures are estimated to occur in up to 90% of mammal species.[25] As polygyny is the most common form of polygamy among vertebrates (including humans), it has been studied far more extensively than polyandry or polygynandry.

Polyandry

[edit]
The anglerfishHaplophryne mollis is polyandrous. This female is trailing the atrophied remains of males she has encountered.
Main article:Polyandry in nature

Polyandry occurs when one female gets exclusive mating rights with multiple males. In some species, such asredlip blennies, bothpolygyny andpolyandry are observed.[26]

The males in some deep seaanglerfishes are much smaller than the females. When they find a female they bite into her skin, releasing anenzyme that digests the skin of their mouths and her body and fusing the pair down to the blood-vessel level. The male then slowlyatrophies, losing first his digestive organs, then his brain, heart, and eyes, ending as nothing more than a pair ofgonads, which releasesperm in response tohormones in the female's bloodstream indicatingegg release. This extremesexual dimorphism ensures that, when the female is ready to spawn, she has a mate immediately available.[27] A single anglerfish female can "mate" with many males in this manner.

Polygynandry

[edit]
Main article:Polygynandry

Polygynandry occurs when multiple males mate indiscriminately with multiple females. The numbers of males and females need not be equal, and in vertebrate species studied so far, there are usually fewer males. Two examples of systems in primates are promiscuous matingchimpanzees andbonobos. These species live in social groups consisting of several males and several females. Each female copulates with many males, and vice versa. In bonobos, the amount of promiscuity is particularly striking because bonobos use sex to alleviate social conflict as well as to reproduce.[28] This mutual promiscuity is the approach most commonly used by spawning animals, and is perhaps the "original fish mating system."[4]: 161  Common examples areforage fish, such asherrings, which form huge matingshoals in shallow water. The water becomes milky with sperm and the bottom is draped with millions of fertilised eggs.[4]: 161 

Parental investment and reproductive success

[edit]
Main article:Bateman's principle
Matinggrey slugs, suspended from a slime thread

Female and male sexual behaviour differ in many species. Often, males are more active in initiating mating, and bear the more conspicuous sexual ornamentation like antlers and colourful plumage. This is a result ofanisogamy, wheresperm are smaller and much less costly (energetically) to produce thaneggs. This difference in physiological cost means that males are more limited by the number of mates they can secure, while females are limited by the quality of genes of her mates, a phenomenon known asBateman's principle.[29] Many females also have extra reproductive burdens in thatparental care often falls mainly, or exclusively, on them. Thus, females are more limited in their potentialreproductive success.[30] In species where males take on more of the reproductive costs, such assea horses andjacanas, the role is reversed, and the females are larger, more aggressive and more brightly coloured than the males.

In hermaphroditic animals, the costs of parental care can be evenly distributed between the sexes, e.g.earthworms. In some species ofplanarians, sexual behaviour takes the form ofpenis fencing. In this form of copulation, the individual that first penetrates the other with the penis, forces the other to be female, thus carrying the majority of the cost of reproduction.[31] Post mating,banana slugs will some times gnaw off their partners penis as an act ofsperm competition called apophallation.[32] This is costly as they must heal, and spend more energy courting conspecifics that can act as male and female. A hypothesis suggests these slugs may be able to compensate the loss of the male function by directing energy that would have been put towards it to the female function.[33] In thegrey slug, the sharing of cost leads to a spectacular display, where the mates suspend themselves high above the ground from a slime thread, ensuring none of them can refrain from taking on the cost of egg-bearer.[34]

Seasonality

[edit]
Main article:Seasonal breeder
Brain corals typicallyspawning in connection with the full moon every August

Many animal species have specific mating (or breeding) periods e.g. (seasonal breeding) so that offspring are born or hatch at an optimal time. In marine species with limited mobility andexternal fertilisation likecorals,sea urchins andclams, the timing of the common spawning is the only externally visible form of sexual behaviour. In areas with continuously highprimary production, some species have a series of breeding seasons throughout the year. This is the case with most primates (who are primarily tropical and subtropical animals). Some animals (opportunistic breeders) breed dependent upon other conditions in their environment aside from time of year.

Mammals

[edit]

Mating seasons are often associated with changes to herd or group structure, and behavioural changes, including territorialism amongst individuals. These may be annual (e.g.wolves), biannual (e.g.dogs) or more frequently (e.g. horses). During these periods, females of most mammalian species are more mentally and physically receptive to sexual advances, a period scientifically described asoestrus but commonly described as being "in season" or "in heat". Sexual behaviour may occur outside oestrus,[35] and such acts as do occur are not necessarily harmful.[36]

Some mammals (e.g. domestic cats, rabbits and camelids) are termed"induced ovulators". For these species, the female ovulates due to an external stimulus during, or just prior to, mating, rather than ovulating cyclically or spontaneously. Stimuli causing induced ovulation include the sexual behaviour of coitus, sperm and pheromones. Domestic cats havepenile spines. Upon withdrawal of acat's penis, the spines rake the walls of the female'svagina, which may cause ovulation.[37][38]

Amphibians

[edit]

For many amphibians, an annual breeding cycle applies, typically regulated by ambient temperature, precipitation, availability of surface water and food supply. This breeding season is accentuated in temperate regions, in boreal climate the breeding season is typically concentrated to a few short days in the spring. Some species, such as theRana clamitans (green frog), spend from June to August defending their territory. In order to protect these territories, they use five vocalizations.[39]

Fish

[edit]

Like manycoral reef dwellers, theclownfish spawn around the time of the full moon in the wild. In a group of clownfish, there is a strict dominance hierarchy. The largest and most aggressive female is found at the top. Only two clownfish, a male and a female, in a group reproduce through external fertilisation. Clownfish are sequential hermaphrodites, meaning that they develop into males first, and when they mature, they become females. If the female clownfish is removed from the group, such as by death, one of the largest and most dominant males will become a female. The remaining males will move up a rank in the hierarchy.

Motivation

[edit]

Variousneurohormones stimulate sexual wanting in animals. In general, studies have suggested thatdopamine is involved in sexual incentive motivation,oxytocin andmelanocortins in sexual attraction, andnoradrenaline in sexual arousal.[40]Vasopressin is also involved in the sexual behaviour of some animals.[41]

Neurohormones in the mating systems of voles

[edit]

The mating system ofprairie voles ismonogamous; after mating, they form a lifelong bond. In contrast,montane voles have apolygamous mating system. When montane voles mate, they form no strong attachments, and separate after copulation. Studies[42] on the brains of these two species have found that it is two neurohormones and their respective receptors that are responsible for these differences in mating strategies. Male prairie voles release vasopressin after copulation with a partner, and an attachment to their partner then develops. Female prairie voles release oxytocin after copulation with a partner, and similarly develop an attachment to their partner.

Neither male nor female montane voles release high quantities of oxytocin or vasopressin when they mate. Even when injected with these neurohormones, their mating system does not change. In contrast, if prairie voles are injected with the neurohormones, they may form a lifelong attachment, even if they have not mated. The differing response to the neurohormones between the two species is due to a difference in the number of oxytocin and vasopressin receptors. Prairie voles have a greater number of oxytocin and vasopressin receptors compared to montane voles, and are therefore more sensitive to those two neurohormones. It's believed that it's the quantity of receptors, rather than the quantity of the hormones, that determines the mating system and bond-formation of either species.[citation needed]

Oxytocin and rat sexual behaviour

[edit]

Mother rats experience apostpartum estrus which makes them highly motivated to mate. However, they also have a strong motivation to protect their newly born pups. As a consequence, the mother rat solicits males to the nest but simultaneously becomes aggressive towards them to protect her young. If the mother rat is given injections of an oxytocinreceptor antagonist, they no longer experience these maternal motivations.[43]

Prolactin influences social bonding in rats.[43]

Oxytocin and primate sexual behaviour

[edit]

Oxytocin plays a similar role in non-human primates as it does in humans.

Grooming, sex, and cuddling frequencies correlate positively with levels of oxytocin. As the level of oxytocin increases so does sexual motivation. While oxytocin plays a major role in parent child relationships, it is also found to play a role in adult sexual relationships. Its secretion affects the nature of the relationship or if there will even be a relationship at all.[citation needed][44]

Studies have shown that oxytocin is higher in monkeys in lifelong monogamous relationships compared to monkeys which are single. Furthermore, the oxytocin levels of the couples correlate positively; when the oxytocin secretion of one increases, the other one also increases. Higher levels of oxytocin are related to monkeys expressing more behaviours such as cuddling, grooming and sex, while lower levels of oxytocin reduce motivation for these activities.[citation needed]

Research on oxytocin's role in the animal brain suggests that it plays less of a role in behaviours of love and affection than previously believed. "When oxytocin was first discovered in 1909, it was thought mostly to influence a mother's labour contractions and milk let-down. Then, in the 1990s, research with prairie voles found that giving them a dose of oxytocin resulted in the formation of a bond with their future mate (Azar, 40)." Oxytocin has since been treated by the media as the sole player in the "love and mating game" in mammals. This view, however, is proving to be false as, "most hormones don't influence behaviour directly. Rather, they affect thinking and emotions in variable ways (Azar, 40)." There is much more involved in sexual behaviour in the mammalian animal than oxytocin and vasopressin can explain.[45][46][47][48][49][50][51][52][53][54]

Pleasure

[edit]

It is often assumed that animals do not have sex for pleasure, or alternatively thathumans,pigs,bonobos (and perhapsdolphins and one or two more species of primates) are the only species that do. This is sometimes stated as "animals mate only for reproduction". This view is considered a misconception by some scholars.[55][56]Jonathan Balcombe argues that the prevalence of non-reproductive sexual behaviour in certain species suggests that sexual stimulation is pleasurable. He also points to the presence of theclitoris in some female mammals, and evidence for female orgasm in primates.[57] On the other hand, it is impossible to know the subjective feelings of animals,[40] and the notion thatnon-human animals experience emotions similar to humans is a contentious subject.[58][59][60][61]

A 2006Danish Animal Ethics Council report,[62] which examined current knowledge of animal sexuality in the context of legal queries concerning sexual acts by humans, has the following comments, primarily related to domestically common animals:

Even though the evolution-related purpose of mating can be said to be reproduction, it is not actually the creating of offspring which originally causes them to mate. It is probable that they mate because they are motivated for the actual copulation, and because this is connected with a positive experience. It is therefore reasonable to assume that there is some form of pleasure or satisfaction connected with the act. This assumption is confirmed by the behaviour of males, who in the case of many species are prepared to work to get access to female animals, especially if the female animal is in oestrus, and males who for breeding purposes are used tohaving sperm collected become very eager, when the equipment they associate with the collection is taken out. . . . There is nothing infemale mammals' anatomy or physiology that contradicts thatstimulation of the sexual organs and mating is able to be a positive experience. For instance, the clitoris acts in the same way as with women, and scientific studies have shown that the success of reproduction is improved by stimulation of clitoris on (among other species) cows and mares in connection with insemination, because it improves the transportation of the sperm due to contractions of the inner genitalia. This probably also applies to female animals of other animal species, and contractions in the inner genitals are seen e.g. also during orgasm for women. It is therefore reasonable to assume that sexual intercourse may be linked with a positive experience for female animals.

Koinophilia

[edit]
Main article:Koinophilia

Koinophilia is the love of the "normal" or phenotypically common (from the Greekκοινός,koinós, meaning "usual" or "common").[63] The term was introduced to scientific literature in 1990, and refers to the tendency of animals seeking a mate to prefer that mate not to have any unusual, peculiar or deviant features.[63] Similarly, animals preferentially choose mates with lowfluctuating asymmetry.[64] However, animal sexual ornaments can evolve through runaway selection, which is driven by (usually female) selection for non-standard traits.[65]

Interpretation bias

[edit]

The field of study of sexuality in non-human species was a long-standingtaboo.[66][unreliable source?] In the past, researchers sometimes failed to observe, miscategorised or misdescribed sexual behaviour whichdid not meet their preconceptions—their bias tended to support what would now be described as conservative sexual mores. An example of overlooking behaviour relates to descriptions ofgiraffe mating:

When nine out of ten pairings occur between males, "[e]very male that sniffed a female was reported as sex, whileanal intercourse withorgasm between males was only [categorized as] 'revolving around'dominance, competition or greetings."[66]

In the 21st century, liberal social or sexual views are often projected upon animal subjects of research. Popular discussions of bonobos are a frequently cited example. Current research frequently expresses views such as that of theNatural History Museum at the University of Oslo, which in 2006 held an exhibition on animal sexuality:

Many researchers have described homosexuality as something altogether different from sex. They must realise that animals can have sex with who they will, when they will and without consideration to a researcher's ethical principles.[66]

Other animal activities may be misinterpreted due to the frequency and context in which animals perform the behaviour. For example, domestic ruminants display behaviours such as mounting and head-butting. This often occurs when the animals are establishing dominance relationships and are not necessarily sexually motivated. Careful analysis must be made to interpret what animal motivations are being expressed by those behaviours.[67]

Types of sexual behaviour

[edit]

Reproductive sexual behaviour

[edit]

Copulation

[edit]
Main article:Copulation (zoology)

Copulation is the union of the male and femalesex organs, the sexual activity specifically organized to transmit male sperm into the body of the female.[68]

Cuckoldry

[edit]
Small malebluegill sunfishescuckold large males by adoptingsneaker strategies.
See also:Cuckoldry in fish

Alternative male strategies which allow small males to engage incuckoldry can develop in species such as fish wherespawning is dominated by large and aggressive males. Cuckoldry is a variant ofpolyandry, and can occur withsneak spawners. A sneak spawner is a male that rushes in to join the spawning rush of a spawning pair.[69] A spawning rush occurs when a fish makes a burst of speed, usually on a near vertical incline, releasinggametes at the apex, followed by a rapid return to the lake or sea floor or fish aggregation.[70] Sneaking males do not take part in courtship. In salmon and trout, for example,jack males are common. These are small silvery males that migrate upstream along with the standard, large, hook-nosed males and that spawn by sneaking intoredds to release sperm simultaneously with a mated pair. This behaviour is anevolutionarily stable strategy for reproduction, because it is favoured by natural selection just like the "standard" strategy of large males.[71]

Hermaphroditism

[edit]
Femalegroupers change their sex to male if no male is available.
See also:Sequential hermaphroditism

Hermaphroditism occurs when a given individual in a species possesses both male and female reproductive organs, or can alternate between possessing first one, and then the other. Hermaphroditism is common in invertebrates but rare in vertebrates. It can be contrasted withgonochorism, where each individual in a species is either male or female, and remains that way throughout their lives. Most fish are gonochorists, but hermaphroditism is known to occur in 14 families ofteleost fishes.[72]

Usually hermaphrodites aresequential, meaning they canswitch sex, usually from female to male (protogyny). This can happen if a dominant male is removed from a group of females. The largest female in the harem can switch sex over a few days and replace the dominant male.[72] This is found amongstcoral reef fishes such asgroupers,parrotfishes andwrasses. As an example, mostwrasses areprotogynous hermaphrodites within a haremic mating system.[73][74] It is less common for a male to switch to a female (protandry).[4]: 162  A common example of a protandrous species areclownfish—if the larger, dominant female dies, in many cases, the reproductive male gains weight and becomes the female.[75][76] Hermaphroditism allows for complex mating systems. Wrasses exhibit three different mating systems: polygynous,lek-like, and promiscuous mating systems.[77]

Sexual cannibalism

[edit]
Main article:Sexual cannibalism
Araneus diadematus – cannibalistic mating behaviour

Sexual cannibalism is a behaviour in which a female animal kills and consumes the male before, during, or after copulation. Sexual cannibalism confers fitness advantages to both the male and female.[78] Sexual cannibalism is common among insects, arachnids[79] andamphipods.[79] There is also evidence of sexual cannibalism ingastropods andcopepods.[80]

Sexual coercion

[edit]
Main article:Sexual coercion among animals
During mating, the maleLeopard typically immobilizes the female

Sexual coercion behaviour during mating has been documented in a variety of species. In someherbivorous herd species, or species where males and females are very different in size, the maledominates sexually by force and size.[citation needed]

Some species of birds have been observed combining sexual intercourse with violence; these includeducks,[81][82] andgeese.[83] Femalewhite-fronted bee-eaters are subjected to copulation. When females emerge from their nest burrows, males sometimes force them to the ground and mate with them. Such sexual coercion are made preferentially on females who are laying and who may therefore lay eggs fertilized by the male.[84]

It has been reported that young male elephants in South Africa sexually coerced and killed rhinoceroses.[85] This interpretation of the elephants' behaviour was disputed by one of the original study's authors, who said there was "nothing sexual about these attacks".[86]

In elephant seals' sexual intercourse, male elephant seals have been known to have relations with penguins, attempting to copulate with them "several times with periods of rest in between". The birds remained pinned down for the duration. For female elephant seals, physical injury happens very often. Mating leads to 1 in every 1,000 female elephant seals getting killed.[87]

Parthenogenesis

[edit]

Parthenogenesis is a form of asexual reproduction in which growth and development of embryos occur without fertilisation.[88] Technically, parthenogenesis is not a behaviour, however, sexual behaviours may be involved.

Whip-tailed lizard females have the ability to reproduce through parthenogenesis and as such males are rare and sexual breeding non-standard. Females engage in "pseudocopulation"[89] to stimulateovulation, with their behaviour following their hormonal cycles; during low levels of oestrogen, these (female) lizards engage in "masculine" sexual roles. Those animals with currently high oestrogen levels assume "feminine" sexual roles. Lizards that perform the courtship ritual have greater fecundity than those kept in isolation due to an increase in hormones triggered by the sexual behaviours. So, even though asexual whiptail lizards populations lack males, sexual stimuli still increase reproductive success. From anevolutionary standpoint these females are passing their full genetic code to all of their offspring rather than the 50% of genes that would be passed in sexual reproduction.[citation needed]

It is rare to find true parthenogenesis in fishes, where females produce female offspring with no input from males. All-female species include the Texassilverside,Menidia clarkhubbsi[90] and a complex of Mexicanmollies.[4]: 162 

Parthenogenesis has been recorded in 70 vertebrate species[91] includinghammerhead sharks,[92]blacktip sharks,[93] amphibians[94]and lizards.[95]

Unisexuality

[edit]

Unisexuality occurs when a species is all-male or all-female. Unisexuality occurs in some fish species and can take complex forms.Squalius alburnoides, a minnow found in several river basins in Portugal and Spain, appears to be an all-male species. The existence of this species illustrates the potential complexity of mating systems in fish. The species originated as a hybrid between two species and isdiploid but not hermaphroditic. It can havetriploid andtetraploid forms, including all-female forms that reproduce mainly throughhybridogenesis.[96]

Others

[edit]
A dog mates with a coyote to produce adog-coyote hybrid.
  • Interbreeding:Hybrid offspring can result from the mating of two organisms of distinct but closely related parent species, although the resulting offspring is not alwaysfertile. According toAlfred Kinsey, genetic studies on wild animal populations have shown a "large number" of inter-species hybrids.[97]
  • Prostitution: There are reports that animals occasionally engage inprostitution. A small number of pair-bonded females within a group of penguins took nesting material (stones) after copulating with a non-partner male. The researcher stated "I was watching opportunistically, so I can't give an exact figure of how common it really is."[98] It has been reported that "bartering of meat for sex ... forms part of the social fabric of a troop of wild chimps living in the Tai National Park in the Côte d'Ivoire."[99]
  • Pavlovian conditioning: The sexualisation of objects or locations is recognised in the animal breeding world. For example, male animals may becomesexually aroused upon visiting a location where they have been allowed to have sex before, or upon seeing a stimulus previously associated with sexual activity such as anartificial vagina.[100] Sexual preferences for certain cues can be artificially induced in rats by pairing scents or objects with their early sexual experiences.[101] The primary motivation of this behaviour isPavlovian conditioning, and the association is due to a conditioned response (or association) formed with a distinctive "reward".[101]
  • Viewing images: A study using four adult male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) showed that malerhesus macaques will give up a highly valued item, juice, to see images of the faces orperineum of high-status females.[102] Encouraging captivepandas to mate is problematic. Showing young male pandas "panda pornography" is credited with a recent population boom among pandas in captivity in China. One researcher attributed the success to the sounds on the recordings.[103]
  • Copulatory wounding andtraumatic insemination: Injury to a partner's genital tract during mating occurs in at least 40 taxa, ranging from fruit flies to humans. However, it often goes unnoticed due to its cryptic nature and because of internal wounds not visible outside.[104]

Non-reproductive sexual behaviour

[edit]
Main article:Non-reproductive sexual behaviour in animals

There is a range of behaviours that animals perform that appear to be sexually motivated but which can not result in reproduction. These include:

  • Masturbation: Some species, both male and female, masturbate, both when partners are available and otherwise.[105][106][107]
  • Oral sex: Several species engage in bothautofellatio and oral sex. This has been documented inbrown bears,[108]Tibetan macaques,[109]wolves,[110]goats,primates,bats,[111][112]cape ground squirrels[113] andsheep. In thegreater short-nosed fruit bat, copulation by males is dorsoventral and the females lick the shaft or the base of the male's penis, but not the glans which has already penetrated the vagina. While the females do this, the penis is not withdrawn and research has shown a positive relationship between length of the time that the penis is licked and the duration of copulation. Post copulation genital grooming has also been observed.[114]
  • Homosexuality: Same-sex sexual behaviour occurs in a range of species, especially in social species, particularly in marine birds and mammals, monkeys, and thegreat apes. As of 1999[update], the scientific literature contained reports of homosexual behaviour in at least 471 wild species.[115] Organisers of theAgainst Nature? exhibit stated that "homosexuality has been observed among 1,500 species, and that in 500 of those it is well documented."[116] Besides humans,sheep are the only known mammal to exhibit exclusive homosexual behavior.[117]
A maleblack and white tegu mounts a female that has been dead for two days and attempts to mate.[118]
  • Genital-genital rubbing: This is sexual activity in which one animal rubs his or her genitals against the genitals of another animal. This is stated to be the "bonobo's most typical sexual pattern, undocumented in any other primate".[119][120]
  • Inter-species mating: Some animals opportunistically mate with individuals of another species.[121]
  • Sex involving juveniles: Malestoats (Mustela erminea) will sometimes mate with infant females of their species.[122] This is a natural part of their reproductive biology—they have adelayed gestation period, so these females give birth the following year when they are fully grown. Juvenile malecommon chimpanzees have been recorded mounting and copulating with immature chimps. Infants inbonobo societies are often involved in sexual behaviour.[123]
  • Necrophilia: This describes when an animal engages in a sexual act with a dead animal. It has been observed in mammals, birds, reptiles and frogs.[124]
  • Bisexuality: This describes when an animal shows sexual behaviour towards both males and females.
  • Extended female sexuality: This is when females mate with males outside of their conceptive period.[125][22]

Seahorse

[edit]

Seahorses, once considered to be monogamous species with pairs mating for life, were described in a 2007 study as "promiscuous, flighty, and more than a little bit gay".[126] Scientists at 15aquaria studied 90 seahorses of three species. Of 3,168 sexual encounters, 37% were same-sex acts. Flirting was common (up to 25 potential partners a day of both sexes); only one species (the British spiny seahorse) included faithful representatives, and for these 5 of 17 were faithful, 12 were not. Bisexual behaviour was widespread and considered "both a great surprise and a shock", with big-bellied seahorses of both sexes not showing partner preference. 1,986 contacts were male-female, 836 were female-female and 346 were male-male.[126]

Bonobo

[edit]

Amongbonobos, males and femalesengage in sexual behaviour with the same and the opposite sex, with females being particularly noted for engaging in sexual behaviour with each other and at up to 75% of sexual activity being non-reproductive, as being sexually active does not necessarily correlate with their ovulation cycles.[119] Sexual activity occurs between almost all ages and sexes of bonobo societies.[127][128] Primatologist Frans de Waal believes that bonobos use sexual activity to resolve conflict between individuals.[28][129] Immature bonobos, contrariwise, perform genital contact when relaxed.[128]

Macaque

Similar same-sex sexual behaviours occur in both male and female macaques.[130] It is thought to be done for pleasure as an erect male mounts and thrusts upon or into another male.[130][131] Sexual receptivity can also be indicated by red faces and shrieking.[130] Mutual ejaculation after a combination of anal intercourse and masturbation has also been witnessed, although it may be rare.[131] In comparison to socio-sexual behaviours such as dominance displays, homosexual mounts last longer, happen in series, and usually involve pelvic thrusting.[130]

Females are also thought to participate for pleasure as vulvar, perineal, and anal stimulation is part of these interactions. The stimulation can come from their own tails, mounting their partner, thrusting or a combination of these.[132]

Dolphin

[edit]

Malebottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs to follow or restrict the movement of a female for weeks at a time, waiting for her to become sexually receptive. The same pairs have also been observed engaging inintense sexual play with each other. Janet Mann, a professor of biology and psychology at Georgetown University, argues[133] that the common same-sex behaviour among maledolphin calves is about bond formation and benefits the species evolutionarily. Studies have shown the dolphins later in life are bisexual and the male bonds forged from homosexuality work for protection as well as locating females with which to reproduce.[133]

In 1991, an English man was prosecuted for allegedly having sexual contact with a dolphin.[134] The man was found not guilty after it was revealed at trial that the dolphin was known to tow bathers through the water by hooking his penis around them.[134]

Hyena

[edit]

The femalespotted hyena hasa unique urinary-genital system, closely resembling the penis of the male, called apseudo-penis. Dominance relationships with strong sexual elements are routinely observed between related females. They are notable for using visible sexual arousal as a sign of submission but not dominance in males as well as females (females have a sizeable erectileclitoris).[135] It is speculated that to facilitate this, theirsympathetic andparasympathetic nervous systems may be partially reversed in respect to their reproductive organs.[136]

Mating behaviour

[edit]
See also:Mating call

Vertebrates

[edit]

Mammals

[edit]
Further information:Mammalian reproduction andSocial monogamy in mammalian species

Mammals mate by vaginal copulation. To achieve this, the male usually mounts the female from behind.[137] The female may exhibitlordosis in which she arches her back ventrally to facilitate entry of the penis, which is particularly present in elephants,felids, and rodents. Amongst the land mammals, other than humans, only bonobos mate in a face-to-face position,[138][better source needed] as the females' anatomy seems to reflect,[119] although ventro-ventral copulation has also been observed inRhabdomys.[139] Some sea mammals copulate in a belly-to-belly position.[140][141] Somecamelids mate in a lying-down position.[142] In most mammals ejaculation occurs after multiple intromissions,[143] but in most primates, copulation consists of one brief intromission.[144] In most ruminant species, a singlepelvic thrust occurs during copulation.[145][146] In most deer species, a copulatory jump also occurs.[147][148]

During mating, a "copulatory tie" occurs in mammals such asfossas,[149]canids with the exception ofAfrican wild dogs,[150] andJapanese martens.[151] A "copulatory lock" also occurs in some primate species, such asGalago senegalensis.[152]

The copulatory behaviour of many mammalian species is affected bysperm competition.[153]

Some females have concealed fertility, making it difficult for males to evaluate if a female is fertile - humans are amongst these species. This is costly as ejaculation expends much energy.[22]

Invertebrates

[edit]
See also:Mating of gastropods
Courtinggarden snails. The one on the left has fired alove dart into the one on the right.
A malestar coral releases sperm into the water.

Invertebrates are oftenhermaphrodites. Some hermaphroditicland snails begin mating with an elaborate tactile courting ritual. The two snails circle around each other for up to six hours, touching with their tentacles, and biting lips and the area of the genital pore, which shows some preliminary signs of the eversion of the penis. As the snails approach mating, hydraulic pressure builds up in the blood sinus surrounding an organ housing a sharpened dart. The dart is made ofcalcium carbonate orchitin, and is called alove dart. Each snail manoeuvres to get its genital pore in the best position, close to the other snail's body. Then, when the body of one snail touches the other snail's genital pore, it triggers the firing of the love dart.[154] After the snails have fired their darts, they copulate and exchange sperm as a separate part of the mating progression. The love darts are covered with a mucus that contains ahormone-like substance that facilitates the survival of the sperm.[155][156]

Penis fencing is a mating behaviour engaged in by certain species offlatworm, such asPseudobiceros bedfordi. Species which engage in the practice are hermaphroditic, possessing both eggs and sperm-producing testes.[157] The species "fence" using two-headed dagger-like penises which are pointed, and white in colour. One organism inseminates the other. The sperm is absorbed through pores in the skin, causing fertilisation.

Corals can be bothgonochoristic (unisexual) andhermaphroditic, each of which can reproduce sexually and asexually. Reproduction also allows corals to settle new areas. Corals predominantly reproducesexually. 25% ofhermatypic corals (stony corals) form single sex (gonochoristic) colonies, while the rest arehermaphroditic.[158] About 75% of all hermatypic corals "broadcast spawn" by releasinggametes –eggs andsperm – into the water to spread offspring. The gametes fuse during fertilisation to form a microscopiclarva called aplanula, typically pink and elliptical in shape.[159]Synchronous spawning is very typical on the coral reef and often, even when multiplespecies are present, all corals spawn on the same night. This synchrony is essential so that male and female gametes can meet. Corals must rely on environmental cues, varying from species to species, to determine the proper time to release gametes into the water. The cues involve lunar changes, sunset time, and possibly chemical signalling.[158] Synchronous spawning may form hybrids and is perhaps involved in coralspeciation.[160]

Butterflies spend much time searching for mates. When the male spots a mate, he will fly closer and releasepheromones. He then performs a specialcourtship dance to attract the female. If the female appreciates the dancing she may join him. Then they join their bodies together end to end at theirabdomens. Here, the male passes the sperm to the female's egg-laying tube, which will soon be fertilised by the sperm.[161]

Many animals makeplugs of mucus to seal the female'sorifice after mating. Normally such plugs are secreted by the male, to block subsequent partners. In spiders the female can assist the process.[162] Spider sex is unusual in that males transfer their sperm to the female on small limbs called pedipalps. They use these to pick their sperm up from their genitals and insert it into the female's sexual orifice, rather than copulating directly.[162] On the 14 occasions a sexual plug was made, the female produced it without assistance from the male. On ten of these occasions the male's pedipalps then seemed to get stuck while he was transferring the sperm (which is rarely the case in other species of spider), and he had great difficulty freeing himself. In two of those ten instances, he was eaten as a result.[162]

In the orb-weaving spider speciesZygiella x-notata, individuals engage in a variety of sexual behaviors including male choosiness, mate guarding, and vibrational signaling in courtship.[163][164]

Genetic evidence of interspecies sexual activity in humans

[edit]
Main article:Humanzee

Research intohuman evolution confirms that, in some cases, interspecies sexual activity may have been responsible for the evolution of new species (speciation). Analysis of animalgenes found evidence that, afterhumans had diverged from otherapes, interspecies mating nonetheless occurred regularly enough to change certain genes in the newgene pool.[165] Researchers found that theX chromosomes of humans and chimps may havediverged around 1.2 million years after the other chromosomes. One possible explanation is that modern humans emerged from a hybrid of human and chimp populations.[166] A 2012 study questioned this explanation, concluding that "there is no strong reason to involve complicated factors in explaining the autosomal data".[167][dubiousdiscuss]

Inbreeding avoidance

[edit]
Main article:Inbreeding avoidance

When close relatives mate, progeny may exhibit the detrimental effects ofinbreeding depression. Inbreeding depression is predominantly caused by thehomozygous expression of recessive deleterious alleles.[168] Over time, inbreeding depression may lead to the evolution ofinbreeding avoidance behaviour. Several examples of animal behaviour that reduce mating of close relatives and inbreeding depression are described next.

Reproductively active femalenaked mole-rats tend to associate with unfamiliar males (usually non-kin), whereas reproductively inactive females do not discriminate.[169] The preference of reproductively active females for unfamiliar males is interpreted as an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding.

When mice inbreed with close relatives in their natural habitat, there is a significant detrimental effect on progeny survival.[170] In the house mouse, themajor urinary protein (MUP) gene cluster provides a highly polymorphic scent signal of genetic identity that appears to underliekin recognition andinbreeding avoidance. Thus there are fewer matings between mice sharing MUPhaplotypes than would be expected if there were random mating.[171]

Meerkat females appear to be able to discriminate the odour of their kin from the odour of their non-kin.[172] Kin recognition is a useful ability that facilitates both cooperation among relatives and the avoidance of inbreeding. When mating does occur between meerkat relatives, it often results ininbreeding depression. Inbreeding depression was evident for a variety of traits: pup mass at emergence from the natal burrow, hind-foot length, growth until independence and juvenile survival.[173]

Thegrey-sided vole (Myodes rufocanus) exhibits male-biased dispersal as a means of avoiding incestuous matings.[174] Among those matings that do involve inbreeding the number of weaned juveniles in litters is significantly smaller than that from non-inbred litters indicating inbreeding depression.

In natural populations of the birdParus major (great tit), inbreeding is likely avoided by dispersal of individuals from their birthplace, which reduces the chance of mating with a close relative.[175] Dispersing to avoid inbreeding is a common behavior amongst animals, such asfelids andcanids, although inbreeding can still occur, albeit rarely.

Toads displaybreeding site fidelity, as do manyamphibians. Individuals that return to natal ponds to breed will likely encountersiblings as potential mates. Althoughincest is possible,Bufo americanus siblings rarely mate. These toads likely recognise and actively avoid close kins as mates. Advertisement vocalisations by males appear to serve as cues by which females recognise their kin.[176]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^Kent, Michael (2000).Advanced biology.Oxford University Press. pp. 250–253.ISBN 978-0-19-914195-1.
  2. ^Thorpe, Showick; Thorpe, Edgar (2009).General Studies Manual.Pearson Education India. p. 17.ISBN 9788131721339.
  3. ^Wickler, Wolfgang; Lorenz; Konrad; Kacher, Hermann (1974). "The sexual code : the social behaviour of animals and men".{{cite journal}}:Cite journal requires|journal= (help)
  4. ^abcdeMoyle PB and Cech JJ (2004)Fishes, An Introduction to Ichthyology. 5th Ed, Benjamin Cummings.ISBN 978-0-13-100847-2
  5. ^Berglund A (1997)"Mating systems and sex allocation" Pages 237–265 in JJ Godon, ed.Behavioural ecology of teleost fishes. Oxford University Press.ISBN 0-19-850503-5.
  6. ^"Pigeon".Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved26 August 2014.
  7. ^Lily Whiteman (13 February 2013)."Animal Attraction: The Many Forms of Monogamy in the Animal Kingdom".National Science Foundation.Archived from the original on 25 July 2014. Retrieved26 August 2014.
  8. ^Ågren, G.; Zhou, Q.; Zhong, W. (1989). "Ecology and social behaviour of Mongolian gerbils,Meriones unguiculatus, at Xilinhot, Inner Mongolia, China".Animal Behaviour.37:11–27.doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90002-X.S2CID 53152632.
  9. ^abBirkhead, T.R.; Møller, A.P. (1995). "Extra-pair copulations and extra-pair paternity in birds".Animal Behaviour.49 (3):843–848.doi:10.1016/0003-3472(95)80217-7.S2CID 53156057.
  10. ^abcReichard, U.H. (2002)."Monogamy – a variable relationship"(PDF).Max Planck Research.3:62–67. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 14 May 2011. Retrieved24 April 2013.
  11. ^Westneat, D. F.; Stewart, I. R. K. (2003). "Extra-Pair Paternity in Birds: Causes, Correlates, and Conflict".Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics.34:365–396.doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132439.
  12. ^Birkhead, T.R. & Møller, A.P. (1996) "Monogamy and sperm competition in birds". In J. M. Black (Ed.),Partnerships in Birds: The Study of Monogamy,pp. 323–343, Oxford: Oxford University PressISBN 0-19-854860-5.
  13. ^Owens, I. P. F.; Hartley, I. R. (1998)."Sexual dimorphism in birds: Why are there so many different forms of dimorphism?".Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.265 (1394):397–407.doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0308.JSTOR 50849.PMC 1688905.
  14. ^Solomon, N. G.; Keane, B.; Knoch, L. R.; Hogan, P. J. (2004). "Multiple paternity in socially monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster)".Canadian Journal of Zoology.82 (10):1667–1671.Bibcode:2004CaJZ...82.1667S.doi:10.1139/z04-142.
  15. ^Reichard, U.H. (2003). Monogamy: Past and present. In U.H. Reichard and C. Boesch (Eds.), Monogamy: Mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans, and other mammals, pp. 3–25, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,ISBN 0521819733.
  16. ^abcBarash, D.P. & Lipton, J.E. (2001). The Myth of Monogamy. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman and Company,ISBN 0805071369.
  17. ^Angier, Natalie (21 August 1990)."Mating for Life? It's Not for the Birds of the Bees".The New York Times.Archived from the original on 5 November 2007. Retrieved14 February 2017.
  18. ^Morell, V. (1998). "Evolution of Sex: A New Look at Monogamy".Science.281 (5385):1982–1983.doi:10.1126/science.281.5385.1982.PMID 9767050.S2CID 31391458.
  19. ^Mcneil, Jeremy N (1986). "Calling Behavior: Can It Be Used to Identify Migratory Species of Moths".The Florida Entomologist.69 (1):78–84.doi:10.2307/3494746.JSTOR 3494746.
  20. ^Robert Sapolsky (2005)."Biology and Human Behavior: The Neurological Origins of Individuality, 2nd edition". The Teaching Company.Archived from the original on 16 February 2014. Retrieved10 November 2010.
  21. ^This section and examples taken fromRobert Sapolsky (1998)Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers, W.H. Freeman and Co.,ISBN 0-7167-3210-6, pp. 140–141.
  22. ^abcFürtbauer, Ines; Heistermann, Michael; Schülke, Oliver; Ostner, Julia (10 August 2011)."Concealed Fertility and Extended Female Sexuality in a Non-Human Primate (Macaca assamensis)".PLOS ONE.6 (8): e23105.Bibcode:2011PLoSO...623105F.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023105.ISSN 1932-6203.PMC 3154278.PMID 21853074.
  23. ^Haartman, L. V. (1951). "Successive Polygamy".Behaviour.3:256–273.doi:10.1163/156853951X00296.
  24. ^Silverin, B. (1979). "Effects of long-acting testosterone administration on testes in free living pied flycatchersFicedula hypoleuca".Endokrinologie.74 (2):141–146.PMID 583410.
  25. ^Aloise King, Edith D.; Banks, Peter B.; Brooks, Robert C. (2011). "Sexual conflict in mammals: consequences for mating systems and life history".Mammal Review.43 (1) (published January 2013):47–58.doi:10.1111/j.1365-2907.2011.00200.x.
  26. ^Marraro, CH; Nursall JR (1983). "The reproductive periodicity and behaviour of Ophioblennius atlanticus at Barbados".J Zool.61 (2):317–325.doi:10.1139/z83-042.
  27. ^Theodore W. Pietsch (1975). "Precocious sexual parasitism in the deep sea ceratioid anglerfish, Cryptopsaras couesi Gill".Nature.256 (5512):38–40.Bibcode:1975Natur.256...38P.doi:10.1038/256038a0.S2CID 4226567.
  28. ^ab"Homosexual Activity Among Animals Stirs Debate". 23 July 2004. Archived fromthe original on 13 November 2015. Retrieved18 November 2018.
  29. ^Bateman, A.J. (1948), "Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila",Heredity,2 (Pt. 3):349–368,doi:10.1038/hdy.1948.21,PMID 18103134
  30. ^Trivers, R.L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.),Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.ISBN 0-435-62157-2
  31. ^Hermaphrodites duel for manhoodArchived 26 March 2008 at theWayback Machine,Science News Online. Accessed 14 March 2009.
  32. ^Miller, Brooke L. W (2007).Sexual conflict and partner manipulation in the banana slug,Ariolimax dolichophallus. Santa Cruz: University of California.Archived from the original on 13 February 2015. Retrieved13 February 2015.
  33. ^Backeljau, Thierry; Jordaens, Kurt; Dillen, Lobke (2007)."Effects of mating, breeding system and parasites on reproduction in hermaphrodites: Pulmonate gastropods (Mollusca)".Animal Biology.57 (2):137–195.doi:10.1163/157075607780377965. Retrieved18 November 2018.
  34. ^Leonard, J. L. (5 May 2006)."Sexual selection: lessons from hermaphrodite mating systems".Integrative and Comparative Biology.46 (4):349–367.doi:10.1093/icb/icj041.PMID 21672747.
  35. ^For example, masturbation, trial mounting, and other behaviours are regularly seen in male animals out of season
  36. ^Denmark, Det Dyreetiske Råd,Udtalelse om menneskers seksuelle omgang med dyrArchived 4 October 2021 at theWayback Machine (Copenhagen: Justitsministeriet, November 2006), p. 24. "Slimhinden i hundyrets vagina og dyrets adfærd er under indflydelse af dets brunstcyklus. Det betyder, at dyret er fysisk og mentalt mere parat til seksuelle aktiviteter på nogle tidspunkter end på andre. Men dette er ikke ensbetydende med, at den seksuelle aktivitet vil være forbundet med skader, angst og lidelse, hvis den foregår udenfor brunstperioden." (Translation: "The mucous membrane in the female animal's vagina and the animal's behaviour is under influence of its rut cycle. That means that the animal is physically and mentally more ready for sexual activities at some times than at others. But this does not mean that sexual activity will lead to injuries, fear or suffering, if it happens outside the rut period.")
  37. ^Virginia Douglass Hayssen; Ari Van Tienhoven (1993).Asdell's Patterns of Mammalian Reproduction: A Compendium of Species-specific Data. Cornell University Press.ISBN 978-0-8014-1753-5. Retrieved27 September 2013.
  38. ^Aronson, L. R.; Cooper, M. L. (1967)."Penile spines of the domestic cat: their endocrine-behavior relations"(PDF).Anatomical Record.157 (1):71–78.doi:10.1002/ar.1091570111.PMID 6030760.S2CID 13070242. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 19 March 2015.
  39. ^Wells, Kentwood D (November 1978). "Territoriality in the green frog (Rana clamitans): Vocalizations and agonistic behaviour".Animal Behaviour.26 (4):1051–1054.doi:10.1016/0003-3472(78)90094-5.S2CID 54344542.
  40. ^abGeorgiadis, J. R.; Kringelbach, M. L. & Pfaus, J. G. (2012)."Sex for fun: a synthesis of human and animal neurobiology".Nature Reviews Urology.9 (9):486–498.doi:10.1038/nrurol.2012.151.PMID 22926422.S2CID 13813765.
  41. ^Bear, Mark F. (2007).Neuroscience, exploring the brain. Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. pp. 544–545.ISBN 978-0781760034.
  42. ^Inoue, K.; Burkett, J. P.; Young, L. J. (2013)."Neuroanatomical distribution of μ-opioid receptor mRNA and binding in monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and non-monogamous meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus)".Neuroscience.244:122–133.doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.035.PMC 4327842.PMID 23537838.
  43. ^abKennett, J.E.; McKee, D.T. (2012)."Oxytocin: An emerging regulator of prolactin secretion in the female rat".Journal of Neuroendocrinology.24 (3):403–412.doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02263.x.PMC 3288386.PMID 22129099.
  44. ^Snowdon, Charles T.; Pieper, Bridget A.; Boe, Carla Y.; Cronin, Katherine A.; Kurian, Aimee V.; Ziegler, Toni E. (2010)."Variation in oxytocin is related to variation in affiliative behavior in monogamous, pairbonded tamarins".Hormones and Behavior.58 (4):614–618.doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.014.hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0012-2863-F.PMC 2933949.PMID 20600045.
  45. ^Lambert, K. (2011).The Lab Rat Chronicles. New York: penguin group. pp. 151–172.
  46. ^Azar, B. (March 2011)."Oxytocin's other side".Science Watch.42 (3): 40.Archived from the original on 14 November 2012. Retrieved4 November 2012.
  47. ^Aubert, Y.; Gustison, M. L.; Gardner, L. A.; Bohl, M. A.; Lange, J. R.; Allers, K. A.; Sommer, B.; Datson, N. A.; Abbott, D. H. (2012)."Flibanserin and 8-OH-DPAT Implicate Serotonin in Association between Female Marmoset Monkey Sexual Behavior and Changes in Pair-Bond Quality".The Journal of Sexual Medicine.9 (3):694–707.doi:10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02616.x.PMC 5898967.PMID 22304661.
  48. ^Gil, M.; Bhatt, R.; Picotte, K. B.; Hull, E. M. (2011)."Oxytocin in the medial preoptic area facilitates male sexual behavior in the rat".Hormones and Behavior.59 (4):435–443.doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.12.012.PMC 3081415.PMID 21195714.
  49. ^Scott, Graham (2004).Essential Animal Behavior. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 166–197.ISBN 978-0632057993.
  50. ^Agrati, D.; Fernández-Guasti, A.; Ferreño, M.; Ferreira, A. (2011). "Coexpression of sexual behavior and maternal aggression: The ambivalence of sexually active mother rats toward male intruders".Behavioral Neuroscience.125 (3):446–451.doi:10.1037/a0023085.PMID 21517149.
  51. ^McHenry, J. A.; Bell, G. A.; Parrish, B. P.; Hull, E. M. (2012)."Dopamine D1 receptors and phosphorylation of dopamine- and cyclic AMP-regulated phosphoprotein-32 in the medial preoptic area are involved in experience-induced enhancement of male sexual behavior in rats".Behavioral Neuroscience.126 (4):523–529.doi:10.1037/a0028707.PMC 3409344.PMID 22708956.
  52. ^Matsushita, Hiroaki; Tomizawa, Kazuhito; Okimoto, Naoki; Nishiki, Tei-ichi; Ohmori, Iori; Matsui, Hideki (1 October 2010)."Oxytocin mediates the antidepressant effects of mating behavior in male mice".Neuroscience Research.68 (2):151–153.doi:10.1016/j.neures.2010.06.007.ISSN 0168-0102.PMID 20600375.S2CID 207152048.
  53. ^Phan, Joseph; Alhassen, Lamees; Argelagos, Allan; Alhassen, Wedad; Vachirakorntong, Benjamin; Lin, Zitong; Sanathara, Nayna; Alachkar, Amal (12 August 2020)."Mating and parenting experiences sculpture mood-modulating effects of oxytocin-MCH signaling".Scientific Reports.10 (1): 13611.Bibcode:2020NatSR..1013611P.doi:10.1038/s41598-020-70667-x.ISSN 2045-2322.PMC 7423941.PMID 32788646.
  54. ^Matsushita, Hiroaki; Sasaki, Yuya; Yunoki, Aya; Matsuji, Ayuka; Latt, Hein Min; Onishi, Kazunari; Tomizawa, Kazuhito; Matsui, Hideki (1 August 2022)."Antidepressant-like effect of male mating behavior through oxytocin-induced CREB signaling".Neuroscience Research.181:74–78.doi:10.1016/j.neures.2022.04.002.ISSN 0168-0102.PMID 35421523.S2CID 248089550.
  55. ^Hull, Elaine M.; Meisel, R. L.; Sachs, B. D. (2002)."Male sexual behavior"(PDF).Hormones, Brain and Behavior.1:3–137.doi:10.1016/B978-012532104-4/50003-2.ISBN 9780125321044.Archived(PDF) from the original on 1 August 2016. Retrieved13 August 2016.
  56. ^Jeffrey Moussaieff Masson (21 October 2009).When Elephants Weep: The Emotional Lives of Animals. Random House Publishing Group.ISBN 978-0-307-57420-6. Retrieved28 May 2013.
  57. ^Balcombe, J. (2009)."Animal pleasure and its moral significance".Applied Animal Behaviour Science.118 (3): 212.doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.012.S2CID 16847348.Archived from the original on 2 May 2020. Retrieved6 July 2019.
  58. ^Dawkins, M. (2000)."Animal minds and animal emotions".American Zoologist.40 (6):883–888.doi:10.1668/0003-1569(2000)040[0883:amaae]2.0.co;2.S2CID 86157681. Archived fromthe original on 20 March 2020.
  59. ^Panksepp, J. (1982). "Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions".Behavioral and Brain Sciences.5 (3):407–422.doi:10.1017/S0140525X00012759.S2CID 145746882.
  60. ^"Emotions help animals to make choices (press release)". University of Bristol. 2010.Archived from the original on 4 October 2021. Retrieved26 October 2013.
  61. ^Jacky Turner; Joyce D'Silva, eds. (2006).Animals, Ethics and Trade: The Challenge of Animal Sentience. Earthscan.ISBN 9781844072545. Retrieved26 October 2013.
  62. ^Denmark, Det Dyreetiske Råd,Udtalelse om menneskers seksuelle omgang med dyrArchived 4 October 2021 at theWayback Machine (Copenhagen: Justitsministeriet, November 2006), p. 23–24. "Selv om det evolutionsmæssige formål med at parre sig kan siges at være reproduktion, er det ikke selve det, at dyrene får afkom, der i første omgang får dem til at parre sig. Det er til gengæld sandsynligt, at de parrer sig, fordi de er motiverede for selve parringsakten, og at denne er forbundet med en positiv oplevelse. Det er derfor rimeligt at antage, at der er en eller anden form for behag eller tilfredsstillelse forbundet med akten. Denne antagelse bekræftes af adfærden hos handyr, der for mange arters vedkommende er parate til at arbejde for at få adgang til hundyr, især hvis hundyret er i brunst, og handyr der i avlsøjemed er vant til at få tappet sæd – de viser stor ivrighed, når det udstyr, de forbinder med sædopsamlingen, tages frem. . . . Der er intet ved hunpattedyrenes anatomi eller fysiologi, der modsiger, at stimulation af kønsorganerne og parring skulle kunne være en positiv oplevelse – fx fungerer klitoris på samme måde som hos kvinder. Videnskabelige undersøgelser har vist, at reproduktionssuccesen forbedres ved stimulation af klitoris på bl.a. køer og hopper i forbindelse med insemination, fordi det forbedrer sædtransporten pga. sammentrækninger af de indre kønsdele. Dette gælder sandsynligvis også hundyr af andre dyrearter, og sammentrækninger i de indre kønsdele ses fx også under orgasme hos kvinder. Det er derfor rimeligt at antage, at det seksuelle samvær kan være forbundet med en positiv oplevelse for hundyrene."
  63. ^abKoeslag, J.H. (1990). "Koinophilia groups sexual creatures into species, promotes stasis, and stabilizes social behaviour".Journal of Theoretical Biology.144 (1):15–35.Bibcode:1990JThBi.144...15K.doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80297-8.PMID 2200930.
  64. ^Moller, A.P.; Pomiankowski, A (1993). "Fluctuating asymmetry and sexual selection".Genetica.89 (1–3):267–279.doi:10.1007/bf02424520.S2CID 40071460.
  65. ^Dawkins, Richard (1986). The Blind Watchmaker. Longman, London. Published in Penguin Books 1988, 1991, and 2006. Chapter 8, Explosions and Spirals.
  66. ^abc"1,500 animal species practice homosexuality". News-medical.net. 23 October 2006.Archived from the original on 8 August 2014. Retrieved19 February 2007.
  67. ^Katz, L. S.; McDonald, T. J. (1992). "Sexual behavior of farm animals".Theriogenology.38 (2):239–253.doi:10.1016/0093-691X(92)90233-H.PMID 16727133.
  68. ^Knobil E., Neill J.D. (Eds).The physiology of reproduction. Academic Press, 3nd edition, 2005
  69. ^Streak spawningArchived 10 February 2015 at theWayback Machine Fishbase Glossary. Retrieved 11 February 2011.
  70. ^Spawning rushArchived 10 February 2015 at theWayback Machine Fishbase Glossary. Retrieved 11 February 2011.
  71. ^Gross MR (1984) "Sunfish, salmon, and the evolution of alternative reproductive strategies and tactics in fishes". Pages 55–75 in GW Potts and RJ Wottoon, eds.Fish reproduction: Strategies and tactics. Academic Press.
  72. ^abShapiro DY (1984) "Sex reversal and sociodemographics processes in coral reef fishes" Pages 103–116 in GW Potts and RK Wootoon, eds.,Fish reproduction: Strategies and tactics, Academic Press.
  73. ^Robertson, D.R.; R.R. Warner (1978). "Sexual patterns in the labroid fishes of the Western Caribbean II: the parrotfishes (Scaridae)".Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology.255 (255):1–26.doi:10.5479/si.00810282.255.
  74. ^Kazancioglu, E.; S.H. Alonzo (August 2010)."A comparative analysis of sex change in Labridae supports the size advantage hypothesis".Evolution.64 (8):2254–2264.doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01016.x.PMID 20394662.S2CID 8184412.
  75. ^Buston, Peter M. (May 2004)."Territory Inheritance in Clownfish".Proceedings of the Royal Society B.271 (Suppl 4):S252 –S254.doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0156.PMC 1810038.PMID 15252999.
  76. ^Buston, P. (2004). "Does the Presence of Non-Breeders Enhance the Fitness of Breeders? An Experimental Analysis in the Clown AnemonefishAmphiprion percula".Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.57 (1):23–31.Bibcode:2004BEcoS..57...23B.doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0833-2.S2CID 24516887.
  77. ^Colin, P.L.; L. J. Bell (1992)."Aspects of the spawning of labrid and scarid fishes (Pisces, Labroidei) at Enewetak Atoll, Marshall Islands with notes on other families (corrected reprint.)".Environmental Biology of Fishes.33 (3):330–345.doi:10.1007/BF00005881.
  78. ^Zuk, Marlene (December 2016)."Mates with Benefits: When and How Sexual Cannibalism Is Adaptive".Current Biology.26 (23):R1230 –R1232.Bibcode:2016CBio...26R1230Z.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.10.017.PMID 27923131.
  79. ^abPolis, G.A. (1981). "The evolution and dynamics of intraspecific +4193 predation".Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics.51:225–251.Bibcode:1981AnRES..12..225P.doi:10.1146/annurev.es.12.110181.001301.S2CID 86286304.
  80. ^Bilde, T.; Tuni, C.; Elsayed, R.; Pekár, S.; Toft, S. (2006)."Death feigning in the face of sexual cannibalism".Biology Letters.2 (1):23–5.doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0392.PMC 1617195.PMID 17148316.
  81. ^Bailey, R.O.; Seymour, N. R.; Stewart, G.R. (1978)."Rape behaviour in blue-winged teal"(PDF).Auk.95 (1):188–90.doi:10.2307/4085514.JSTOR 4085514.Archived(PDF) from the original on 29 November 2014. Retrieved5 March 2013.
  82. ^Barash, D. P. (1977). "Sociobiology of Rape in Mallards (Anas platyrhynchos): Responses of the Mated Male".Science.197 (4305):788–789.Bibcode:1977Sci...197..788B.doi:10.1126/science.197.4305.788.PMID 17790773.S2CID 34257131.
  83. ^Mineau, Pierre; Cooke, Fred (1979). "Rape in the Lesser Snow Goose".Behaviour.70 (3):280–291.doi:10.1163/156853979x00098.ISSN 0005-7959.
  84. ^Emlen, S. T.; Wrege, P. H. (2010). "Forced Copulations and Intra-specific Parasitism: Two Costs of Social Living in the White-fronted Bee-eater".Ethology.71:2–29.doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1986.tb00566.x.
  85. ^Siebert, Charles. (8 October 2006)An Elephant Crackup?, Charles Siebert, New York Times Magazine, 8 October 2006Archived 14 November 2012 at theWayback Machine. Nytimes.com. Retrieved on 2011-12-22.
  86. ^"Have elephants begun raping rhinos in the wild?".The Straight Dope. 25 July 2008.Archived from the original on 27 December 2012. Retrieved5 March 2013.
  87. ^Cite error: The named referencenineteen was invoked but never defined (see thehelp page).
  88. ^Wininger, J. David (2004), "Parthenogenetic Stem Cells",Handbook of Stem Cells, Elsevier, pp. 635–637,doi:10.1016/b978-012436643-5/50072-9,ISBN 9780124366435
  89. ^Hiskey, D. (31 May 2011)."New Mexico whiptail lizards are all females".Archived from the original on 13 February 2015. Retrieved13 February 2015.
  90. ^Echelle AA, Echelle AF, Crozier CD (1983). "Evolution of an all-female fish,Menidia clarkhubbsi (Atherinidae)".Evolution.37 (4):772–784.doi:10.2307/2407918.JSTOR 2407918.PMID 28568119.
  91. ^Harmon, K. (2010)."No sex needed: All-female lizard species cross their chromosomes to make babies".Scientific American. Retrieved13 February 2015.
  92. ^"Captive shark had 'virgin birth'".BBC News. 23 May 2007.Archived from the original on 17 December 2008. Retrieved23 December 2008.
  93. ^"'Virgin birth' for aquarium shark".Metro.co.uk. 10 October 2008.Archived from the original on 11 October 2008. Retrieved10 October 2008.
  94. ^Halliday, Tim R.; Adler, Kraig, eds. (1986).Reptiles & Amphibians. Torstar Books. p. 101.ISBN 978-0-920269-81-7.
  95. ^Walker, Brian (11 November 2010)."Scientists discover unknown lizard species at lunch buffet".CNN.Archived from the original on 13 September 2018. Retrieved11 November 2010.
  96. ^Alves MJ, Collarea-Pereira MJ, Dowling TE, Coelho MM (2002). "The genetics of maintenance of an all-male lineage in theSqualius alburnoides complex".Journal of Fish Biology.60 (3):649–662.Bibcode:2002JFBio..60..649A.doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2002.tb01691.x.
  97. ^Kinsey, Alfred (1953).Sexual Behavior in the Human Female. W.B. Saunders Company. p. 504.
  98. ^"Penguins are turning to prostitution". BBC. 26 February 1998.Archived from the original on 6 August 2017. Retrieved12 June 2008.
  99. ^Connor, Steve (8 April 2009)."Sex for meat – how chimps seduce their mates".The Independent. London.Archived from the original on 7 May 2022.
  100. ^Det Dyreetiske Råd (2006)."Udtalelse om menneskers seksuelle omgang med dyr"(PDF). Justitsministeriet. Archived fromthe original(PDF) on 27 February 2012.'Selv om det evolutionsmæssige formål med at parre sig kan siges at være reproduktion, er det ikke selve det, at dyrene får afkom, der i første omgang får dem til at parre sig. Det er til gengæld sandsynligt, at de parrer sig, fordi de er motiverede for selve parringsakten, og at denne er forbundet med en positiv oplevelse. Det er derfor rimeligt at antage, at der er en eller anden form for behag eller tilfredsstillelse forbundet med akten. Denne antagelse bekræftes af adfærden hos handyr, der for mange arters vedkommende er parate til at arbejde for at få adgang til hundyr, især hvis hundyret er i brunst, og handyr der i avlsøjemed er vant til at få tappet sæd – de viser stor ivrighed, når det udstyr, de forbinder med sædopsamlingen, tages frem.' [Translation to English] 'Although the evolutionary purpose of mating can be said to be reproduction, it is not the very fact that animals have offspring that causes them to mate in the first place. Rather, it is likely that they mate because they are motivated by the act of mating itself and that this is associated with a positive experience. It is therefore reasonable to assume that there is some form of pleasure or satisfaction associated with the act. This assumption is confirmed by the behaviour of male animals, which for many species are prepared to work to gain access to females, especially if the female is in heat, and male animals which for breeding purposes are used to having their semen collected - they show great eagerness when the equipment they associate with semen collection is taken out.'{{cite journal}}:Cite journal requires|journal= (help)
  101. ^abPfaus, J. G.; Kippin, T. E.; Coria-Avila, G. A.; Gelez, H.; Afonso, V. M.; Ismail, N.; Parada, M. (2012). "Who, what, where, when (and maybe even why)? How the experience of sexual reward connects sexual desire, preference, and performance".Archives of Sexual Behavior.41 (1):31–62.doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9935-5.PMID 22402996.S2CID 12421026.
  102. ^Deaner M. O.; Khera A. V.; Platt M. L. (2005)."Monkeys pay per view: adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques".Current Biology.15 (6):543–548.Bibcode:2005CBio...15..543D.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.044.PMID 15797023.S2CID 1746276.
  103. ^Gray, Denis D. (27 November 2006)Porn sparks panda baby boom in China: Research — and blue movies — attributed to record-high birth rate in 2006Archived 4 May 2020 at theWayback Machine. Associated Press (via NBC News). Retrieved on 22 December 2011.
  104. ^Klaus Reinhardt, Nils Anthes, and Rolanda Lange (2015)"Copulatory Wounding and Traumatic Insemination"Archived 11 January 2016 at theWayback MachineCold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015; 7: a017582
  105. ^Würsig, Bernd; Orbach, Dara N. (25 September 2023).Sex in Cetaceans: Morphology, Behavior, and the Evolution of Sexual Strategies. Springer Nature.ISBN 978-3-031-35651-3.
  106. ^Watson, P. F. (1978).Artificial breeding of non-domestic animals: (the proceedings of a symposium held at the Zoological Society of London on 7 and 8 September 1977). Academic Press for the Zoological Society of London.ISBN 978-0-12-613343-1. Retrieved9 February 2013.
  107. ^Balcombe, Jonathan P. (2011).The Exultant Ark: A Pictorial Tour of Animal Pleasure. University of California Press. pp. 89–.ISBN 978-0-520-26024-5.
  108. ^These Bears Are Having Lots Of Oral Sex, And Scientists Think They Know Why (The Huffington Post)Archived 14 July 2014 at theWayback Machine By: Grenoble, Ryan.
  109. ^Hideshi Ogawa (2006).Wily Monkeys: Social Intelligence of Tibetan Macaques. Kyoto University Press. pp. 4–.ISBN 978-1-920901-97-4.
  110. ^Fox, M. W. (1972). "The Social Significance of Genital Licking in the Wolf,Canis lupus".Journal of Mammalogy.53 (3):637–640.doi:10.2307/1379064.JSTOR 1379064.
  111. ^Sugita, Norimasa."Homosexual Fellatio: Erect Penis Licking between Male Bonin Flying Foxes Pteropus pselaphon."Archived 6 November 2017 at theWayback Machine PLOS One 11.11 (2016): e0166024.
  112. ^Tan, M.; Jones, G.; Zhu, G.; Ye, J.; Hong, T.; Zhou, S.; Zhang, S.; Zhang, L. (2009). Hosken, David (ed.)."Fellatio by Fruit Bats Prolongs Copulation Time".PLOS ONE.4 (10): e7595.Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.7595T.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007595.PMC 2762080.PMID 19862320.
  113. ^Waterman, J. M. (2010). Briffa, Mark (ed.)."The Adaptive Function of Masturbation in a Promiscuous African Ground Squirrel".PLOS ONE.5 (9): e13060.Bibcode:2010PLoSO...513060W.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013060.PMC 2946931.PMID 20927404.
  114. ^Tan, Min; Gareth Jones; Guangjian Zhu; Jianping Ye; Tiyu Hong; Shanyi Zhou; Shuyi Zhang; Libiao Zhang (28 October 2009). Hosken, David (ed.)."Fellatio by Fruit Bats Prolongs Copulation Time".PLOS ONE.4 (10): e7595.Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.7595T.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007595.PMC 2762080.PMID 19862320.
  115. ^Bagemihl, Bruce (1999).Biological Exuberance: Animal Homosexuality and Natural Diversity. St. Martin's Press. p. 673.
  116. ^"Oslo gay animal show draws crowds".BBC News. 19 October 2006.Archived from the original on 29 October 2006. Retrieved19 October 2006.
  117. ^Poiani, Aldo; Dixson, A. F. (2010).Animal Homosexuality: A Biosocial Perspective. Cambridge, England:Cambridge University Press. p. 179.ISBN 9781139490382.This makes O. aries (ram) only the second mammal known, apart from humans, capable of displaying exclusive homosexuality.
  118. ^Sazima, I. (2015)."Corpse bride irresistible: a dead female tegu lizard (Salvator merianae) courted by males for two days at an urban park in South-eastern Brazil".Herpetology Notes.8:15–18.Archived from the original on 23 February 2015. Retrieved24 February 2015.
  119. ^abcde Waal FB (1995). "Bonobo sex and society".Scientific American.272 (3):82–8.Bibcode:1995SciAm.272c..82W.doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0395-82.PMID 7871411.Perhaps the bonobo's most typical sexual pattern, undocumented in any other primate, is genito-genital rubbing (or GG rubbing) between adult females. One female facing another clings with arms and legs to a partner that, standing on both hands and feet, lifts her off the ground
  120. ^Paoli, T.; Palagi, E.; Tacconi, G.; Tarli, S. B. (2006). "Perineal swelling, intermenstrual cycle, and female sexual behavior in bonobos (Pan paniscus)".American Journal of Primatology.68 (4):333–347.doi:10.1002/ajp.20228.PMID 16534808.S2CID 25823290.
  121. ^Walker, Matt (2 May 2008)Science/Nature 'Sex pest' seal attacks penguinArchived 3 May 2008 at theWayback Machine. BBC News. Retrieved on 15 February 2011.
  122. ^Doncarlos, Michael W.; Petersen, Jay S.; Tilson, Ronald L. (1986). "Captive biology of an asocial mustelid;Mustela erminea".Zoo Biology.5 (4):363–370.doi:10.1002/zoo.1430050407.
  123. ^Dawkins, Richard (2004). "Chimpanzees".The Ancestor's Tale. Houghton Mifflin.ISBN 978-1-155-16265-2.
  124. ^de Mattos Brito, L. B.; Joventino, I. R.; Ribeiro, S. C.; Cascon, P. (2012)."Necrophiliac behavior in the "cururu" toad,Rhinella jimi Steuvax, 2002, (Anura, Bufonidae) from Northeastern Brazil"(PDF).North-Western Journal of Zoology.8 (2): 365.Archived(PDF) from the original on 5 February 2015. Retrieved5 February 2015.
  125. ^Thornhill, R.; Gangestad, S. W. (2008).The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality. USA: Oxford University Press. pp. 37–55.
  126. ^abSimon De BruxellesPromiscuous and bisexual — the 'faithful' seahorse has a secret sex life . Timesonline.co.uk. 31 January 2007. Retrieved on 22 December 2011.
  127. ^Zihlman, A. L.; Hunter, W. S. (1972). "A biomechanical interpretation of the pelvis of Australopithecus".Folia Primatologica.18 (1):1–19.doi:10.1159/000155465.PMID 4658666.
  128. ^abHashimoto, Chie (1997). "Context and Development of Sexual Behavior of Wild Bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Zaire".International Journal of Primatology.18 (1):1–21.doi:10.1023/a:1026384922066.ISSN 0164-0291.S2CID 22744816.
  129. ^Clay, Zanna; de Waal, Frans B.M (2014)."Sex and strife: post-conflict sexual contacts in bonobos"(PDF).Behaviour.152 (3–4):313–334.doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003155.Archived(PDF) from the original on 5 February 2018. Retrieved18 November 2018.
  130. ^abcdLeca, Jean-Baptiste; Gunst, Noëlle; Vasey, Paul L. (28 May 2014). "Male Homosexual Behavior in a Free-Ranging All-Male Group of Japanese Macaques at Minoo, Japan".Archives of Sexual Behavior.43 (5):853–861.doi:10.1007/s10508-014-0310-6.ISSN 0004-0002.PMID 24867180.S2CID 30375191.
  131. ^abWallen, K.; Parsons, W. A. (1997)."Sexual behavior in same-sexed nonhuman primates: Is it relevant to understanding human homosexuality?".Annual Review of Sex Research.8:195–223.doi:10.1080/10532528.1997.10559922.PMID 10051894. Retrieved18 November 2018.
  132. ^Vasey, Paul L.; Duckworth, Nadine (2006)."Sexual Reward via Vulvar, Perineal, and Anal Stimulation: A Proximate Mechanism for Female Homosexual Mounting in Japanese Macaques | Request PDF".Archives of Sexual Behavior.35 (5):523–532.doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9111-x.PMID 17048107.S2CID 24498074. Retrieved18 November 2018.
  133. ^abCentral Park Zoo's gay penguins ignite debateArchived 12 June 2012 at theWayback Machine. Sfgate.com (7 February 2004). Retrieved on 22 December 2011.
  134. ^abUnwin, Brian (22 January 2008)."'Tougher laws' to protect friendly dolphins".The Telegraph. London. Archived fromthe original on 27 December 2012. Retrieved4 April 2018.
  135. ^East, Marion L.; Hofer, Heribert; Wickler, Wolfgang (1993). "The erect 'penis' is a flag of submission in a female-dominated society: greetings in Serengeti spotted hyenas".Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.33 (6):355–370.Bibcode:1993BEcoS..33..355E.doi:10.1007/BF00170251.S2CID 23727803.
  136. ^Sapolsky (1998), Why Zebras Don't Get Ulcers, W.H. Freeman and Co.,ISBN 0-7167-3210-6, pp. 127–129.
  137. ^Dewsbury, Donald A. "Patterns of copulatory behavior in male mammals." Quarterly Review of Biology (1972): 1-33.
  138. ^Fortey, I. (2008)."The 15 most bizarre animal mating rituals".Archived from the original on 18 February 2015. Retrieved18 February 2015.
  139. ^Dufour, Claire M.-S.; Pillay, Neville; Ganem, Guila (2015)."Ventro–ventral copulation in a rodent: a female initiative? | Journal of Mammalogy | Oxford Academic".Journal of Mammalogy.96 (5):1017–1023.doi:10.1093/jmammal/gyv106.
  140. ^Diaz, K."Tursiops aduncus Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin". Animal Diversity Web.Archived from the original on 20 October 2016. Retrieved19 October 2016.
  141. ^Adulyanukosol, K.; Thongsukdee, S.; Hara, T.; Arai, N.; Tsuchiya, M. (2007). "Observations of dugong reproductive behavior in Trang Province, Thailand: further evidence of intraspecific variation in dugong behavior".Marine Biology.151 (5):1887–1891.Bibcode:2007MarBi.151.1887A.doi:10.1007/s00227-007-0619-y.S2CID 86253387.
  142. ^"Bactrian & Dromedary Camels".Factsheets. San Diego Zoo Global Library. March 2009. Archived fromthe original on 22 September 2012. Retrieved4 December 2012.
  143. ^Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience. Elsevier Science. 16 April 2010.ISBN 978-0-08-091455-8.
  144. ^L. Alterman; Gerald A. Doyle; M.K. Izard (9 March 2013).Creatures of the Dark. Springer Science & Business Media.ISBN 978-1-4757-2405-9.
  145. ^David E. Noakes (23 April 2009).Arthur's Veterinary Reproduction and Obstetrics. Elsevier Health Sciences UK. pp. 714–.ISBN 978-0-7020-3990-4.
  146. ^Donald M. Broom; Andrew Ferguson Fraser (1 January 2007).Domestic Animal Behaviour and Welfare. CABI. pp. 156–.ISBN 978-1-84593-287-9.
  147. ^Valerius Geist (January 1998).Deer of the World: Their Evolution, Behaviour, and Ecology. Stackpole Books. pp. 72–.ISBN 978-0-8117-0496-0.
  148. ^David M. Shackleton; Royal British Columbia Museum (1999).Hoofed Mammals of British Columbia. UBC Press.ISBN 978-0-7748-0728-9.
  149. ^Lührs, Mia-Lana; Kappeler, Peter M. (2014). "Polyandrous mating in treetops: How male competition and female choice interact to determine an unusual carnivore mating system".Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.68 (6):879–889.Bibcode:2014BEcoS..68..879L.doi:10.1007/s00265-014-1701-3.S2CID 7995708.
  150. ^Dixson, A. F. (1995)."Baculum length and copulatory behaviour in carnivores and pinnipeds (Grand Order Ferae)".Journal of Zoology.235 (1):67–76.doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1995.tb05128.x.Archived(PDF) from the original on 3 June 2016. Retrieved27 August 2020.
  151. ^Tatara, Masaya (1994)."Notes on the breeding ecology and behavior of Japanese martens on Tsushima Islands, Japan".Journal of the Mammalogical Society of Japan.19 (1):67–74.Archived from the original on 8 May 2022. Retrieved27 June 2017.
  152. ^Dixson, Alan F (1987)."Baculum length and copulatory behavior in primates".American Journal of Primatology.13 (1):51–60.doi:10.1002/ajp.1350130107.PMID 31973483.S2CID 84028737.
  153. ^Møller, A. P.; Birkhead, T. R. (1989)."Copulation behaviour in mammals: evidence that sperm competition is widespread"(PDF).Biological Journal of the Linnean Society.38 (2):119–131.doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1989.tb01569.x.
  154. ^Rogers, David; Chase, Ronald (2001). "Dart receipt promotes sperm storage in the garden snail Helix aspersa".Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.50 (2):122–7.Bibcode:2001BEcoS..50..122R.doi:10.1007/s002650100345.S2CID 813656.
  155. ^Chase, R. (2007). "The function of dart shooting in helicid snails".American Malacological Bulletin.23:183–189.doi:10.4003/0740-2783-23.1.183.S2CID 7562144.
  156. ^Chase, R.; Blanchard, K. C. (2006)."The snail's love-dart delivers mucus to increase paternity".Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences.273 (1593):1471–1475.doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3474.PMC 1560308.PMID 16777740.Archived from the original on 18 November 2010. Retrieved24 August 2017.
  157. ^Newman, Leslie."Fighting to mate: flatworm penis fencing".PBS. Archived fromthe original on 14 November 2013. Retrieved24 August 2017.
  158. ^abVeron, J.E.N. (2000).Corals of the World. Vol 3 (3rd ed.). Australia: Australian Institute of Marine Sciences and CRR Qld Pty Ltd.ISBN 978-0-642-32236-4.
  159. ^Barnes, R. and; Hughes, R. (1999).An Introduction to Marine Ecology (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell Science, Inc. pp. 117–141.ISBN 978-0-86542-834-8.
  160. ^Hatta, M.; Fukami, H.; Wang, W.; Omori, M.; Shimoike, K.; Hayashibara, T.; Ina, Y.; Sugiyama, T. (1999)."Reproductive and genetic evidence for a reticulate evolutionary theory of mass spawning corals".Molecular Biology and Evolution.16 (11):1607–1613.doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a026073.PMID 10555292.
  161. ^"Chan Lee Peng, August 2008". Scienceray.com.Archived from the original on 1 February 2013. Retrieved5 March 2013.
  162. ^abc"Sexual Appetite and Animal behaviour – Sex and the single spider".The Economist. Vol. 400, no. 8742. 14 July 2011.Archived from the original on 4 June 2013. Retrieved3 May 2013.
  163. ^Bel-Venner, M.c; Dray, S; Allainé, D; Menu, F; Venner, S (7 January 2008)."Unexpected male choosiness for mates in a spider".Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.275 (1630):77–82.doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1278.PMC 2562406.PMID 17956845.
  164. ^Bel-Venner, M. C.; Venner, S. (1 June 2006)."Mate-guarding strategies and male competitive ability in an orb-weaving spider: results from a field study".Animal Behaviour.71 (6):1315–1322.doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.08.010.ISSN 0003-3472.S2CID 53195526.
  165. ^Patterson, Nick; Daniel J. Richter; Sante Gnerre; Eric S. Lander; David Reich (29 June 2006). "Genetic evidence for complex speciation of humans and chimpanzees".Nature.441 (7097):1103–1108.Bibcode:2006Natur.441.1103P.doi:10.1038/nature04789.ISSN 0028-0836.PMID 16710306.S2CID 2325560.
  166. ^Wade, Nicholas (18 May 2006)Two Splits Between Human and Chimp Lines SuggestedArchived 18 May 2022 at theWayback Machine,New York Times.
  167. ^Yamamichi, M; Gojobori J; Innan H. (January 2012)."An autosomal analysis gives no genetic evidence for complex speciation of humans and chimpanzees".Mol Biol Evol.29 (1):145–56.doi:10.1093/molbev/msr172.PMC 3299331.PMID 21903679.
  168. ^Charlesworth D, Willis JH (2009). "The genetics of inbreeding depression".Nature Reviews Genetics.10 (11):783–96.doi:10.1038/nrg2664.PMID 19834483.S2CID 771357.
  169. ^Clarke FM, Faulkes CG (1999)."Kin discrimination and female mate choice in the naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber".Proc. Biol. Sci.266 (1432):1995–2002.doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0877.PMC 1690316.PMID 10584337.
  170. ^Jiménez JA,Hughes KA, Alaks G, Graham L, Lacy RC (1994). "An experimental study of inbreeding depression in a natural habitat".Science.266 (5183):271–3.Bibcode:1994Sci...266..271J.doi:10.1126/science.7939661.PMID 7939661.
  171. ^Sherborne AL, Thom MD, Paterson S, Jury F, Ollier WE, Stockley P, Beynon RJ, Hurst JL (2007)."The genetic basis of inbreeding avoidance in house mice".Curr. Biol.17 (23):2061–6.Bibcode:2007CBio...17.2061S.doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.041.PMC 2148465.PMID 17997307.
  172. ^Leclaire S, Nielsen JF, Thavarajah NK, Manser M, Clutton-Brock TH (2013)."Odour-based kin discrimination in the cooperatively breeding meerkat".Biology Letters.9 (1): 20121054.doi:10.1098/rsbl.2012.1054.PMC 3565530.PMID 23234867.
  173. ^Nielsen JF, English S, Goodall-Copestake WP, Wang J, Walling CA, Bateman AW, Flower TP, Sutcliffe RL, Samson J, Thavarajah NK, Kruuk LE, Clutton-Brock TH, Pemberton JM (2012). "Inbreeding and inbreeding depression of early life traits in a cooperative mammal".Mol. Ecol.21 (11):2788–804.Bibcode:2012MolEc..21.2788N.doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2012.05565.x.hdl:2263/19269.PMID 22497583.S2CID 36059683.
  174. ^Ishibashi Y, Saitoh T (2008). "Role of male-biased dispersal in inbreeding avoidance in the grey-sided vole (Myodes rufocanus)".Mol. Ecol.17 (22):4887–96.Bibcode:2008MolEc..17.4887I.doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.03969.x.PMID 19140979.S2CID 44992920.
  175. ^Szulkin M, Sheldon BC (2008)."Dispersal as a means of inbreeding avoidance in a wild bird population".Proc. Biol. Sci.275 (1635):703–11.doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0989.PMC 2596843.PMID 18211876.
  176. ^Waldman, B; Rice, JE; Honeycutt, RL (1992)."Kin recognition and incest avoidance in toads".Am. Zool.32:18–30.doi:10.1093/icb/32.1.18.

Bibliography

[edit]

Further reading

[edit]
Sexual behavior of horses

External links

[edit]
General
Invertebrates
Fish
Amphibians
Reptiles
Birds
Mammals
Biological
terms
Sexual
reproduction
Sexuality
Branches
Ethologists
Societies
Journals
Zoophilia
Related subjects
Authority control databases: NationalEdit this at Wikidata
Retrieved from "https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Animal_sexual_behaviour&oldid=1294843620"
Categories:
Hidden categories:

[8]ページ先頭

©2009-2025 Movatter.jp